Displaying 1 - 39 of 39
-
Gerakaki, S., Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Planning speech affects memory of heard words. Poster presented at the 12th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference, Leuven, Belgium.
-
Hintz, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Prediction and production of simple mathematical equations. Poster presented at the 18th Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP 2013), Budapest, Hungary.
Abstract
An important issue in current psycholinguistics is the relationship between the production and comprehension systems. It has been argued that these systems are tightly linked, and that, in particular, listeners use the speech production system to predict upcoming content. We tested this view using a novel version of the visual world paradigm. Participants heard mathematical equations and looked at a clock face showing the numbers 1 to 12. On alternating trials they either heard a complete equation (3+8=11) or they heard the first part (3+8) and had to produce the solution (11, target hereafter) themselves. Participants were encouraged to look at the relevant numbers throughout the trial. On listening trials, the participants typically looked at the target before the onset of target name, and on speaking trials they typically looked at the target before naming it. However, the timing of the looks to the targets was slightly different, with participants looking earlier at the target when they had to speak themselves than when they listened. This suggests that predicting during listening and planning to speak are indeed very similar but not identical. The further methodological and theoretical consequences of the study will be discussed. -
Jongman, S. R., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Sustained attention in language production: An individual differences study. Talk presented at the Nederlandse Vereniging voor Psychonomie (NVP). Egmond aan Zee, The Netherlands. 2013-12-19 - 2013-12-21.
-
Konopka, A. E., Kuchinsky, S., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Does message similarity facilitate sentence formulation?. Poster presented at the 26th CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing [CUNY 2013], Columbia, SC.
Abstract
The generation of an utterance begins with event apprehension and continues with sequential linguistic
encoding of all message elements [2]. The timecourse of formulation, however, is relatively flexible and varies
with the ease of structural encoding [3]. While previous work has shown that syntactic structure may be primed
independently of thematic roles across sentences [1], here we tested whether exposure to conceptually similar
events interacts with structural processes to facilitate the mapping of a message onto a sentence. -
Konopka, A. E., Kuchinsky, S., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Does message similarity facilitate sentence formulation?. Talk presented at the 11th International Symposium of Psycholinguistics. Tenerife, Spain. 2013-03-20 - 2013-03-23.
Abstract
c -
Meyer, A. S., Shao, Z., Randi, M., & Roelofs, A. (2013). The role of selective inhibition in semantic interference tasks. Talk presented at Bangor Meeting of the Experimental Psychology Society. Bangor. 2013-07-03 - 2013-07-05.
Abstract
Recently many authors have stressed that domain-general cognitive processes may affect performance in linguistic tasks. This challenges the traditional view that speaking and listening are fairly modular processes. Going beyond this broad claim, we aim to determine exactly how domain-general processes influence linguistic processes. In the present study we examined the influence of selective inhibition (invoked to suppress responses to potent competitors to target stimuli and taking some time to build up) on performance in two classic word production tasks, the semantic blocking task (naming sets of objects that do vs. do not belong to the same semantic category) and the pictureword interference task (naming pictures accompanied by categorically related vs. unrelated words). Both tasks were completed by the same participants. Analyses of the size of the interference effects for fast and slower responses (using delta plots) and of the correlations of the effect sizes in the two tasks demonstrated that selective inhibition was recruited in both tasks. We propose that the process supported by selective inhibition is lemma selection. We discuss the implications for theories concerning the origin of the interference effects in the two paradigms and the nature of lexical selection processes. -
Meyer, A. S. (2013). What's in it for me? What’s in it for me? Applying adult speech production models to young learners. Talk presented at a Workshop at the University of Leiden. Leiden, The Netherlands. 2013-12.
-
Moers, C., Meyer, A. S., & Janse, E. (2013). Age-related effects of low-level predictability on pronunciation variation in reading aloud for younger and old speakers. Poster presented at Aging and Speech Communication: The 5th International and Interdisciplinary Research Conference, Bloomington, United States.
-
Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Effects of transitional probabilities on word durations in read speech of younger & older speakers. Talk presented at the Centre for Language and Speech Technology colloquium, Radboud University. Nijmegen, The Netherlands. 2013-02.
-
Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Effects of word frequency and transitional probability on reading durations for older adults, younger adults (and children). Talk presented at Speech Production and Aging, graduate seminar, University of California Berkeley. Berkeley, CA, USA. 2013-10.
-
Reifegerste, J., Zwitserlood, P., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). The influence of pseudoword material on the processing of Dutch past-tense verbs. Talk presented at the 8th International Morphological Processing Conference. Cambridge, UK. 2013-06-20 - 2013-06-22.
-
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Piai, V., & Huettig, F. (2013). Constraining the involvement of language production in comprehension: A comparison of object naming and object viewing in sentence context. Talk presented at the 19th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2013]. Marseille, France. 2013-09-02 - 2013-09-04.
-
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Praamstra, P., & Huettig, F. (2013). Anticipating references to objects during sentence comprehension. Talk presented at the Experimental Psychology Society meeting (EPS). Bangor, UK. 2013-07-03 - 2013-07-05.
-
Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Domain-general inhibition helps lexical selection in picture naming: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Poster presented at the 11th Symposium of Psycholinguistics, Tenerife, Spain.
-
Shao, Z., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). individual differences in verbal fluency task performance in older adults. Poster presented at Aging and Speech Communication: The 5th International and Interdisciplinary Research Conference, Bloomington, United States.
-
Shao, Z., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). What do verbal fluency tasks measure?. Poster presented at the 18th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology [ESCOP 2013], Budapest, Hungary.
-
Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). The initiation of speech planning in turn-taking. Talk presented at the 18th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP). Budapest (Hungary). 2013-08-29 - 2013-09-01.
-
Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Is it better to serve a meal than to serve a banquet? Syntactic flexibility and the effects of lexical context in language production. Talk presented at the Experimental Linguistics Talks (ELiTU). Utrecht, The Netherlands. 2013-05-06.
-
Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Is it better to serve a meal than to serve a banquet? Syntactic flexibility and the effects of lexical context in language production. Poster presented at the 12th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference [PIF 2013], Leuven, Belgium.
-
Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Structure selection during sentence production: A role for executive control?. Poster presented at the 26th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing [CUNY 2013], Columbia, SC.
Abstract
Multiple syntactic alternatives are often available to express one message. One of the factors driving the
choice for a syntactic frame is verb bias. This study focuses on the role of verb bias in the process of selecting
a syntactic frame for dative sentences. While some verbs are typically used with one structure (e.g.,
voorleggen [submit] and the prepositional object dative in Dutch), other verbs have a weaker bias towards one
syntactic frame (e.g., voorstellen [propose]): the latter can be used interchangeably in the prepositional object
dative (PD) and double-object dative (DO) construction, and thus allows for some degree of syntactic flexibility
during production. On one view, syntactic flexibility may facilitate production because it enables speakers to fill
the post-verbal sentence slots with either a direct object or an indirect object (the incremental view), while on a
different view, flexibility can lead to competition between structural alternatives, delaying the production of the
sentence until this competition is resolved (the competition view)1. The two views make opposite predictions
regarding the production of sentences featuring verbs with different biases. The incremental view predicts
shorter verb onsets for sentences featuring weak-bias verbs than strong-bias verbs, while the competition view
predicts shorter onsets for sentences with strong-bias verbs. In addition, if the competition view holds,
sentence production may benefit from a mechanism that helps resolve competition between two syntactic
frames by suppressing one frame to enable fast selection of the other frame. We hypothesized that executive
control (EC) can mediate this selection process, facilitating structure selection in the weak verb bias condition. -
Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Structure selection during sentence production: A role for executive control?. Poster presented at the 11th International Symposium of Psycholinguistics, Tenerife, Spain.
Abstract
c -
Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Conceptual and grammatical factors in the production of subject-verb agreement in Dutch. Talk presented at the meeting of the Experimental Psychology Society. London, UK. 2013-01-03 - 2013-01-04.
-
Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Production of subject-verb agreement. Talk presented at the Taalkunde in Nederland-dag (TiN-dag 2013). Utrecht, The Netherlands. 2013-02-09.
-
Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Subject-verb agreement and the role of semantic context. Talk presented at the 34th TABU Dag. Groningen, The Netherlands. 2013-06-13 - 2013-06-14.
-
Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Subject-verb agreement in Dutch and the role of semantic context. Talk presented at the 12th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference. Leuven, Belgium. 2013-05-30 - 2013-05-31.
-
Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2012). Looking at nothing facilitates memory retrieval. Poster presented at Donders Discussions 2012, Nijmegen (NL).
Abstract
When processing visual objects, we integrate visual, linguistic and spatial information to form an episodic trace. Re-activating one aspect of the episodic trace of an object re-activates the entire bundle making all integrated information available. Using the blank screen paradigm [1], researchers observed that upon processing spoken linguistic input, participants tended to make eye movements on a blank screen, fixating locations that were previously occupied by objects mentioned in the linguistic utterance or were related. Ferreira and colleagues [2] suggested that 'looking at nothing' facilitated memory retrieval. However, this claim lacks convincing empirical support. In Experiment 1, Dutch participants looked at four-object-displays. Three objects were related to a spoken target word. Given the target word 'beker' (beaker), the display featured a phonological (a bear), a shape (a bobbin), a semantic (a fork) competitor, and an unrelated distractor (an umbrella). Participants were asked to name the objects as fast as possible. Subsequently, the objects disappeared. Participants fixated the center of the screen and listened to the target word. They had to carry out a semantic judgment task (indicating in which position an object had appeared that was semantically related to the objects) or a visual shape similarity judgment (indicating the position of the object similar in shape to the target). In both conditions, we observed that participants re-fixated the empty target location before responding. The set-up of Experiment 2 was identical except that we asked participants to maintain fixating the center of the screen while listening to the spoken word and responding. Performance accuracy was significantly lower in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1. The results indicate that memory retrieval for objects is impaired when participants are not allowed to look at relevant, though empty locations. [1] Altmann, G. (2004). Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: the 'blank screen paradigm'. Cognition, 93(2), B79-B87. [2] Ferreira, F., Apel, J., & Henderson, J. M. (2008). Taking a new look at looking at nothing. Trends Cogn Sci, 12(11), 405-410. -
Konopka, A. E., Van de Velde, M., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Mapping “easy” and “hard” messages onto language: Conceptual and structural variables jointly affect the timecourse of sentence formulation. Poster presented at the 18th Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2012], Riva del Garda, Italy.
Abstract
Sentence formulation requires mapping pre-verbal messages onto linguistic structures. This message-to-language
mapping is often evaluated in eye-tracking tasks where speakers describe pictured events (The dog chased the mailman).
Speakers can begin sentence formulation by quickly selecting the first-fixated character as the sentential starting point
(lexical incrementality), or generating a rudimentary sentence plan based on their construal of the event gist before
selecting a starting point (hierarchical incrementality; Kuchinsky & Bock, 2010). Lexical incrementality predicts fast
divergence of fixations while hierarchical incrementality predicts slower divergence of fixations to the two characters
within 200ms of picture onset. -
Lesage, E., Morgan, B., Olson, A., Meyer, A. S., & Miall, R. (2012). Disruption of right cerebellum with rTMS blocks predictive language processing. Poster presented at the 42nd annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience [Neuroscience 2012] Poster# 379.07/UU5, New Orleans, LA.
Abstract
Much evidence demonstrates cerebellar involvement in language [1] but a theoretical framework about its precise role is lacking. In cerebellar motor control an influential model ascribes the cerebellum a predictive role [2]. It has been argued that cerebellar nonmotor regions perform similar computations as motor regions, and both are involved in online prediction [2]. We test this hypothesis by administering repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to the right cerebellum, a region implicated in language [3] during a predictive language task. Methods Visual World task [4]: Participants' eye movements were recorded while they listened to sentences and looked at a computer display of an agent and 4 objects, one of which (the target) was mentioned in the sentence. In the Prediction condition the object could be predicted on the basis of the verb; on Control trials it could not. We hypothesised that rTMS to the right cerebellum should make target fixation slower in the Prediction condition, but not in the Control condition. TMS protocol: TMS was delivered between two task blocks. In the cerebellar rTMS group (n = 22) the stimulation site was 1cm down and 3cm right of the inion. Participants received 10min of 1Hz rTMS. In addition, we tested two control groups. In the vertex rTMS group (n = 21), rTMS was applied at the same intensity, duration and frequency as in the cerebellar rTMS group, but over the vertex. In the no stimulation group (n = 22) the coil was placed over the cerebellar stimulation site but no pulses were delivered. Results As hypothesised, participants in the cerebellar rTMS group took longer to fixate the target after TMS in the Prediction condition but not in the Control condition (Block-by-Condition interaction: F(1,21) = 8.848, p = 0.007). This interaction was not found in either the vertex rTMS group (F(1,20) = 0.064, p = 0.802) or the no stimulation group (F(1,21) = 2.461, p = 0.132). Conclusions Here, we show that rTMS to the right cerebellum selectively affects linguistic prediction. These results provide additional evidence that the cerebellum plays a role in language and support theoretical accounts that the cerebellum contributes to nonmotor functions, as it does to motor functions, by online prediction. 1. Strick et al (2009). Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu Rev Neurosci, 32, 413-134 2. Miall et al (1993). Is the cerebellum a Smith predictor. J Mot Behav, 25, 203-216 3. Marien et al (2001). The lateralised linguistic cerebellum: a review and a new hypothesis. Brain and Language, 79, 580-600 4. Altmann & Kamide (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs. Cognition, 73, 247-264 -
Meyer, A. S. (2012). What's in it for me? What's in it for me? Applying adult speech production models to young learners. Talk presented at a workshop at the University of Leiden. Leiden, The Netherlands. 2012-12.
-
Moers, C., Meyer, A. S., & Janse, E. (2012). Effects of transitional probabilities on word durations in read speech of younger & older speakers. Talk presented at the Workshop Fluent Speech: Combining Cognitive and Educational Approaches, Utrecht Institute of Linguistics. Utrecht, The Netherlands. 2012-11-12 - 2012-11-13.
-
Reifegerste, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). The influence of age on the mental representation of polymorphemic words in Dutch. Talk presented at the Conference on Morphological Complexity. London, UK. 2012-01-13 - 2012-01-15.
-
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Praamstra, P., & Huettig, F. (2012). Object shape representations in the contents of predictions for upcoming words. Talk presented at Psycholinguistics in Flanders [PiF 2012]. Berg en Dal, The Netherlands. 2012-06-06 - 2012-06-07.
-
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Praamstra, P., & Huettig, F. (2012). The content of predictions: Involvement of object shape representations in the anticipation of upcoming words. Talk presented at the Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen [TeaP 2012]. Mannheim, Germany. 2012-04-04 - 2012-04-06.
-
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2012). Predicting upcoming meaning involves specific contents and domain-general mechanisms. Talk presented at the 18th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2012]. Riva del Garda, Italy. 2012-09-06 - 2012-09-08.
Abstract
In sentence comprehension, readers and listeners often anticipate upcoming information (e.g., Altmann & Kamide, 1999). We investigated two aspects of this process, namely 1) what is pre-activated when anticipating an upcoming word (the contents of predictions), and 2) which cognitive mechanisms are involved. The contents of predictions at the level of meaning could be restricted to functional semantic attributes (e.g., edibility; Altmann & Kamide, 1999). However, when words are processed other types of information can also be activated, such as object shape representations. It is unknown whether this type of information is already activated when upcoming words are predicted. Forty-five adult participants listened to predictable words in sentence contexts (e.g., "In 1969 Neil Armstrong was the first man to set foot on the moon.") while looking at visual displays of four objects. Their eye movements were recorded. There were three conditions: target present (e.g., a moon and three distractor objects that were unrelated to the predictable word in terms of semantics, shape, and phonology), shape competitor (e.g., a tomato and three unrelated distractors), and distractors only (e.g., rice and three other unrelated objects). Across lists, the same pictures and sentences were used in the different conditions. We found that participants already showed a significant bias for the target object (moon) over unrelated distractors several seconds before the target was mentioned, demonstrating that they were predicting. Importantly, there was also a smaller but significant shape competitor (tomato) preference starting at about a second before critical word onset, consistent with predictions involving the referent’s shape. The mechanisms of predictions could be specific to language tasks, or language could use processing principles that are also used in other domains of cognition. We investigated whether performance in non-linguistic prediction is related to prediction in language processing, taking an individual differences approach. In addition to the language processing task, the participants performed a simple cueing task (after Posner, Nissen, & Ogden, 1978). They pressed one of two buttons (left/right) to indicate the location of an X symbol on the screen. On half of the trials, the X was preceded by a neutral cue (+). On the other half, an arrow cue pointing left (<) or right (>) indicated the upcoming X's location with 80% validity (i.e., the arrow cue was correct 80% of the time). The SOA between cue and target was 500 ms. Prediction was quantified as the mean response latency difference between the neutral and valid condition. This measure correlated positively with individual participants' anticipatory target and shape competitor preference (r = .27; r = .45), and was a significant predictor of anticipatory looks in linear mixed-effects regression models of the data. Participants who showed more facilitation from the arrow cues predicted to a higher degree in the linguistic task. This suggests that prediction in language processing may use mechanisms that are also used in other domains of cognition. References Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247-264. Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Ogden, W. C. (1978). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. In: H.L. Pick, & I.J. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. -
Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Variation in cognitive demands across turn-taking. Poster presented at the 7th International Workshop on Language Production (IWOLP 2012), New York, United States.
-
Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Relative clause processing: Linking clause frequency and reading experience. Poster presented at the 11th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference [PIF 2012], Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
-
Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., Bock, K., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Conceptual and grammatical factors in the production of subject-verb agreement. Poster presented at the 7th International Workshop on Language Production (IWOLP 2012), New York, United States.
-
Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Conceptual and grammatical factors in the production of subject-verb agreement. Talk presented at The 11th edition of the Psycholinguistics in Flanders conference (PiF). Berg en Dal, The Netherlands. 2012-06-06 - 2012-06-07.
-
Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Life after the spoken preamble completion paradigm. Talk presented at the 33th TABU Dag. Groningen, The Netherlands. 2012-06-18 - 2012-06-19.
Share this page