Antje Meyer

Presentations

Displaying 1 - 23 of 23
  • Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2015). Context-dependent employment of mechanisms in anticipatory language processing. Talk presented at the 15th NVP Winter Conference. Egmond aan Zee, The Netherlands. 2015-12-17 - 2015-12-19.
  • Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2015). Doing a production task encourages prediction: Evidence from interleaved object naming and sentence reading. Poster presented at the 28th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Los Angeles (CA, USA).

    Abstract

    Prominent theories of predictive language processing assume that language production processes are used to anticipate upcoming linguistic input during comprehension (Dell & Chang, 2014; Pickering & Garrod, 2013). Here, we explored the converse case: Does a task set including production in addition to comprehension encourage prediction, compared to a task only including comprehension? To test this hypothesis, participants carried out a cross-modal naming task (Exp 1a), a self-paced reading task (Exp1 b) that did not include overt production, and a task (Exp 1c) in which naming and reading trials were evenly interleaved. We used the same predictable (N = 40) and non-predictable (N = 40) sentences in all three tasks. The sentences consisted of a fixed agent, a transitive verb and a predictable or non-predictable target word (The man breaks a glass vs. The man borrows a glass). The mean cloze probability in the predictable sentences was .39 (ranging from .06 to .8; zero in the non-predictable sentences). A total of 162 volunteers took part in the experiment which was run in a between-participants design. In Exp 1a, fifty-four participants listened to recordings of the sentences which ended right before the spoken target word. Coinciding with the end of the playback, a picture of the target word was shown which the participants were asked to name as fast as possible. Analyses of their naming latencies revealed a statistically significant naming advantage of 108 ms on predictable over non-predictable trials. Moreover, we found that the objects’ naming advantage was predicted by the target words’ cloze probability in the sentences (r = .347, p = .038). In Exp 1b, 54 participants were asked to read the same sentences in a self-paced fashion. To allow for testing of potential spillover effects, we added a neutral prepositional phrase (breaks a glass from the collection/borrows a glass from the neighbor) to each sentence. The sentences were read word-by-word, advancing by pushing the space bar. On 30% of the trials, comprehension questions were used to keep up participants' focus on comprehending the sentences. Analyses of their spillover region reading times revealed a numerical advantage (8 ms; tspillover = -1.1, n.s.) in the predictable as compared to the non-predictable condition. Importantly, the analysis of participants' responses to the comprehension questions, showed that they understood the sentences (mean accuracy = 93%). In Exp 1c, the task comprised 50% naming trials and 50% reading trials which appeared in random order. Fifty-four participants named and read the same objects and sentences as in the previous versions. The results showed a naming advantage on predictable over non-predictable items (99 ms) and a positive correlation between the items’ cloze probability and their naming advantage (r = .322, p = .055). Crucially, the post-target reading time analysis showed that with naming trials and reading trials interleaved, there was also a statistically reliable prediction effect on reading trials. Participants were 19 ms faster at reading the spillover region on predictable relative to non-predictable items (tspillover = -2.624). To summarize, although we used the same sentences in all sub-experiments, we observed effects of prediction only when the task set involved production. In the reading only experiment (Exp 1b), no evidence for anticipation was obtained although participants clearly understood the sentences and the same sentences yielded reading facilitation when interleaved with naming trials (Exp 1c). This suggests that predictive language processing can be modulated by the comprehenders’ task set. When the task set involves language production, as is often the case in natural conversation, comprehenders appear to engage in prediction to a stronger degree than in pure comprehension tasks. In our discussion, we will discuss the notion that language production may engage prediction, because being able to predict words another person is about to say might optimize the comprehension process and enable smooth turn-taking.
  • Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2015). Event knowledge and word associations jointly influence predictive processing during discourse comprehension. Poster presented at the 28th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Los Angeles (CA, USA).

    Abstract

    A substantial body of literature has shown that readers and listeners often anticipate information. An open question concerns the mechanisms underlying predictive language processing. Multiple mechanisms have been suggested. One proposal is that comprehenders use event knowledge to predict upcoming words. Other theoretical frameworks propose that predictions are made based on simple word associations. In a recent EEG study, Metusalem and colleagues reported evidence for the modulating influence of event knowledge on prediction. They examined the degree to which event knowledge is activated during sentence comprehension. Their participants read two sentences, establishing an event scenario, which were followed by a final sentence containing one of three target words: a highly expected word, a semantically unexpected word that was related to the described event, or a semantically unexpected and event-unrelated word (see Figure, for an example). Analyses of participants’ ERPs elicited by the target words revealed a three-way split with regard to the amplitude of the N400 elicited by the different types of target: the expected targets elicited the smallest N400, the unexpected and event-unrelated targets elicited the largest N400. Importantly, the amplitude of the N400 elicited by the unexpected but event-related targets was significantly attenuated relative to the amplitude of the N400 elicited by the unexpected and event-unrelated targets. Metusalem et al. concluded that event knowledge is immediately available to constrain on-line language processing. Based on a post-hoc analysis, the authors rejected the possibility that the results could be explained by simple word associations. In the present study, we addressed the role of simple word associations in discourse comprehension more directly. Specifically, we explored the contribution of associative priming to the graded N400 pattern seen in Metusalem et al’s study. We conducted two EEG experiments. In Experiment 1, we reran Metusalem and colleagues’ context manipulation and closely replicated their results. In Experiment 2, we selected two words from the event-establishing sentences which were most strongly associated with the unexpected but event-related targets in the final sentences. Each of the two associates was then placed in a neutral carrier sentence. We controlled that none of the other words in these carrier sentences was associatively related to the target words. Importantly, the two carrier sentences did not build up a coherent event. We recorded EEG while participants read the carrier sentences followed by the same final sentences as in Experiment 1. The results showed that as in Experiment 1 the amplitude of the N400 elicited by both types of unexpected target words was larger than the N400 elicited by the highly expected target. Moreover, we found a global tendency towards the critical difference between event-related and event-unrelated unexpected targets which reached statistical significance only at parietal electrodes over the right hemisphere. Because the difference between event-related and event-unrelated conditions was larger when the sentences formed a coherent event compared to when they did not, our results suggest that associative priming alone cannot account for the N400 pattern observed in our Experiment 1 (and in the study by Metusalem et al.). However, because part of the effect remained, probably due to associative facilitation, the findings demonstrate that during discourse reading both event knowledge activation and simple word associations jointly contribute to the prediction process. The results highlight that multiple mechanisms underlie predictive language processing.
  • Jongman, S. R., Roelofs, A., Meyer, A. S., & Scheper, A. (2015). Sustained attention ability affects language production performance in typical and developmentally impaired children. Poster presented at the 19th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCoP 2015), Paphos, Cyprus.
  • Jongman, S. R., Roelofs, A., Meyer, A. S., & Scheper, A. (2015). Sustained attention ability affects language production performance in typical and developmentally impaired children. Poster presented at the 21st Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2015), Valletta (Malta).
  • Meyer, A. S., Shao, Z., & Van Paridon, J. (2015). Producing complex noun phrases: The roles of word and phrase frequency. Talk presented at the Psychonomic Society's 56th Annual Meeting. Chicago, USA. 2015-11-19 - 2015-11-22.

    Abstract

    Janssen and Barber (2012) reported two studies on the production of complex noun phrases (Spanish and French noun-adjective and noun-noun phrases). They found that production latencies depended only on the frequencies of the phrases, but not on the frequencies of the individual words. This pattern may be seen as evidence for lexical storage of phrases and against the traditional “words & rules” view of the representation of linguistic knowledge. We will discuss a series of experiments on the production of Dutch adjective-noun phrases. We replicated the phrase frequency effect seen by Janssen and Barber, but also found a robust effect of the frequency of the first word of the phrase. We argue that the phrase frequency effect arises during the conceptual preparation of the utterance and that the results are consistent with the view that phrases are composed by combining individual words, in line with the “words & rules” view.
  • Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Probabilistic reduction in reading aloud: A comparison of younger and older adults. Poster presented at the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS 2015), Glasgow, Scotland, UK.

    Abstract

    Frequent and predictable words are generally pronounced with less effort and are therefore acoustically more reduced than less frequent or unpredictable words. Local predictability can be operationalised by Transitional Probability (TP), which indicates how likely a word is to occur given its immediate context. We investigated whether and how probabilistic reduction effects on word durations change with adult age when reading aloud content words embedded in sentences.
    The results showed equally large frequency effects on verb and noun durations for both younger (Mage = 20 years) and older (Mage = 68 years) adults. Backward TP also affected word duration for younger and older adults alike. ForwardTP, however, had no significant effect on word duration in either age group.
    Our results resemble earlier findings of more robust BackwardTP effects compared to ForwardTP effects. Furthermore, unlike often reported decline in predictive processing with aging, probabilistic reduction effects remain stable across adulthood.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., & Praamstra, P. (2015). Tracking double-object naming using the N2pc. Poster presented at the Seventh Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language (SNL 2015), Chicago, IL.
  • Shao, Z., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Word and phrase frequency effects in noun phrase production. Talk presented at the Meeting at the Experimental Psychology Society 2015. London, UK. 2015-01-08.
  • Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). The initiation of speech planning in turn-taking. Poster presented at the 21st Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2015), Malta.
  • Tromp, J., Peeters, D., Hagoort, P., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Combining EEG and virtual reality: The N400 in a virtual environment. Talk presented at the 4th edition of the Donders Discussions (DD, 2015). Nijmegen, Netherlands. 2015-11-05 - 2015-11-06.

    Abstract

    A recurring criticism in the field of psycholinguistics and is the lack of ecological validity of experimental designs. For example, many experiments on sentence comprehension are conducted enclosed booths, where sentences are presented word by word on a computer screen. In addition, very often participants are instructed to make judgments that relate directly to the experimental manipulation. Thus, the contexts in which these processes are studied is quite restricted, which calls into question the generalizability of the results to more naturalistic environments. A possible solution to this problem is the use of virtual reality (VR) in psycholinguistic experiments. By immersing participants into a virtual environment, ecological validity can be increased while experimental control is maintained.
    In the current experiment we combine electroencephalography (EEG) and VR to look at semantic processing in a more naturalistic setting. During the experiment, participants move through a visually rich virtual restaurant. Tables and avatars are placed in the restaurant and participants are instructed to stop at each table and look at the object (e.g. a plate with a steak) in front of the avatar. Then, the avatar will produce an utterance to accompany the object (e.g. “I think this steak is very nice”), in which the noun will either match (e.g. steak) or mismatch (e.g. mandarin) with the item on the table. Based on previous research, we predict a modulation of the N400, which should be larger in the mismatch than the match condition. Implications of the use of virtual reality for experimental research will be discussed.
  • Tromp, J., Hagoort, P., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Indirect request comprehension requires additional processing effort: A pupillometry study. Poster presented at the 19th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCoP 2015), Paphos, Cyprus.
  • Tromp, J., Meyer, A. S., & Hagoort, P. (2015). Pupillometry reveals increased processing demands for indirect request comprehension. Poster presented at the 14th International Pragmatics Conference, Antwerp, Belgium.

    Abstract

    Fluctuations in pupil size have been shown to reflect variations in processing demands during language
    comprehension. Increases in pupil diameter have been observed as a consequence of syntactic anomalies
    (Schluroff 1982), increased syntactic complexity (Just & Carpenter 1993) and lexical ambiguity (Ben-
    Nun 1986). An issue that has not received attention is whether pupil size also varies due to pragmatic
    manipulations. In a pupillometry experiment, we investigated whether pupil diameter is sensitive to
    increased processing demands as a result of comprehending an indirect request versus a statement. During
    natural conversation, communication is often indirect. For example, in an appropriate context, ''It'' cold in
    here'' is a request to shut the window, rather than a statement about room temperature (Holtgraves 1994).
    We tested 49 Dutch participants (mean age = 20.8). They were presented with 120 picture-sentence
    combinations that could either be interpreted as an indirect request (a picture of a window with the
    sentence ''it's hot here'') or as a statement (a picture of a window with the sentence ''it's nice here''). The
    indirect requests were non-conventional, i.e. they did not contain directive propositional content and were
    not directly related to the underlying felicity conditions (Holtgraves 2002). In order to verify that the
    indirect requests were recognized, participants were asked to decide after each combination whether or
    not they heard a request. Based on the hypothesis that understanding this type of indirect utterances
    requires additional inferences to be made on the part of the listener (e.g., Holtgraves 2002; Searle 1975;
    Van Ackeren et al. 2012), we predicted a larger pupil diameter for indirect requests than statements. The
    data were analyzed using linear mixed-effects models in R, which allow for simultaneous inclusion of
    participants and items as random factors (Baayen, Davidson, & Bates 2008). The results revealed a larger
    mean pupil size and a larger peak pupil size for indirect requests as compared to statements. In line with
    previous studies on pupil size and language comprehension (e.g., Just & Carpenter 1993), this difference
    was observed within a 1.5 second window after critical word onset. We suggest that the increase in pupil
    size reflects additional on-line processing demands for the comprehension of non-conventional indirect
    requests as compared to statements. This supports the idea that comprehending this type of indirect
    request requires capacity demanding inferencing on the part of the listener. In addition, this study
    demonstrates the usefulness of pupillometry as a tool for experimental research in pragmatics.
  • Tromp, J., Meyer, A. S., & Hagoort, P. (2015). Pupillometry reveals increased processing demands for indirect request comprehension. Poster presented at the 21st Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2015), Valetta, Malta.

    Abstract

    Fluctuations in pupil size have been shown to reflect variations in processing demands during language
    comprehension. Increases in pupil diameter have been observed as a consequence of syntactic anomalies
    (Schluroff 1982), increased syntactic complexity (Just & Carpenter 1993) and lexical ambiguity (Ben-
    Nun 1986). An issue that has not received attention is whether pupil size also varies due to pragmatic
    manipulations. In a pupillometry experiment, we investigated whether pupil diameter is sensitive to
    increased processing demands as a result of comprehending an indirect request versus a statement. During
    natural conversation, communication is often indirect. For example, in an appropriate context, ''It'' cold in
    here'' is a request to shut the window, rather than a statement about room temperature (Holtgraves 1994).
    We tested 49 Dutch participants (mean age = 20.8). They were presented with 120 picture-sentence
    combinations that could either be interpreted as an indirect request (a picture of a window with the
    sentence ''it's hot here'') or as a statement (a picture of a window with the sentence ''it's nice here''). The
    indirect requests were non-conventional, i.e. they did not contain directive propositional content and were
    not directly related to the underlying felicity conditions (Holtgraves 2002). In order to verify that the
    indirect requests were recognized, participants were asked to decide after each combination whether or
    not they heard a request. Based on the hypothesis that understanding this type of indirect utterances
    requires additional inferences to be made on the part of the listener (e.g., Holtgraves 2002; Searle 1975;
    Van Ackeren et al. 2012), we predicted a larger pupil diameter for indirect requests than statements. The
    data were analyzed using linear mixed-effects models in R, which allow for simultaneous inclusion of
    participants and items as random factors (Baayen, Davidson, & Bates 2008). The results revealed a larger
    mean pupil size and a larger peak pupil size for indirect requests as compared to statements. In line with
    previous studies on pupil size and language comprehension (e.g., Just & Carpenter 1993), this difference
    was observed within a 1.5 second window after critical word onset. We suggest that the increase in pupil
    size reflects additional on-line processing demands for the comprehension of non-conventional indirect
    requests as compared to statements. This supports the idea that comprehending this type of indirect
    request requires capacity demanding inferencing on the part of the listener. In addition, this study
    demonstrates the usefulness of pupillometry as a tool for experimental research in pragmatics.
  • Chu, M., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Name-picture verification as a control measure for object naming: Data from British English speakers. Poster presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010, Manchester, UK.

    Abstract

    The name-picture verification task is often used to assess the difficulty of prelexical processes (object recognition and semantic access) during picture naming. However, whether to use responses from word-picture match or from mismatch trials to index the difficulty of pre-lexical processes is debated. Levelt (2002) argued for the use of mismatch trials because on match trials the printed object name might facilitate picture recognition. However, in a study with speakers of Spanish Stadthagen-Gonzalez et al. (2009) showed that visual and conceptual properties of objects only correlated with the latencies of match responses but not with those of mismatch responses and therefore advocated the use of match responses. The present study aimed to replicate Stadthagen- Gonzalez et al. (2009) findings using native British English speakers and English norms for non-lexical and lexical variables. We replicated the finding that non-lexical variables affected the speed of match, but not mismatch responses. However, in addition, we found that lexical variables also affected the speed of match responses, which means that these latencies need to be interpreted with caution. In other words, neither match nor mismatch responses seem ideally suited to assess the difficulty of pre-lexical processes in picture naming. Levelt, W. J. M. (2002). Picture naming and word frequency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 663–671. Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., Damian, M. F., Pérez, M. A., Bowers, J. S., & Marín, J. (2009). Name-picture verification as a control measure for object naming: A task analysis and norms for a
  • Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Lexical representations of SMS shortcuts. Talk presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010. Manchester, UK. 2010-07-07 - 2010-07-09.

    Abstract

    As the popularity of sending messages electronically increased, so did the necessity of conveying messages more efficiently. A popular way of increasing efficiency is to use shortcuts such as gr8 “great” or cu “see you”. The aim of the study was to investigate whether such abbreviations have their own lexical representations or whether they are recorded into the words they stand for. We used associative masked and overt priming in lexical decision tasks. Primes were text shortcuts and their corresponding words. Targets were words that were associatively related to the prime (e.g., cu/see you – GOODBYE), associatively related to a part of the prime (e.g., cu/see you – LOOK) or unrelated to the prime and any part of the prime (e.g., 4u/for you – GOODBYE; 4u/for you – LOOK). In both tasks, responses were faster to targets preceded by related compared to unrelated primes (shortcuts and words). In the overt priming task, we in addition found faster responses when the target was related to a part of the prime. However, this effect was present only for word but not for shortcut primes. These results indicate that shortcuts have their own lexical representations that are independent of the representations of the individual words they replace.
  • Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Ondrzk vd nwe taal. Talk presented at NWO Conference Bessensap 2010. The Hague, The Netherlands. 2010-06-07.

    Abstract

    Ik laat zien dat het gebruik van sms- en msn-afkortingen geen schadelijke gevolgen heeft op het begrip van geschreven taal zoals blijkt uit een reeks van experimenten. Sterker nog, afkortingen hebben een eigen representatie in ons mentale woordenboek (bijvoorbeeld ‘iig’) en worden niet vertaald naar woorden waaruit ze bestaan (bijvoorbeeld ‘in ieder geval’). Het gebruik van korte tekstberichten via mobiele telefoons en e-mail neemt steeds meer toe en daardoor worden steeds meer afkortingen en acroniemen in de taal gebruikt. Momenteel worden er wereldwijd naar schatting 2000 miljard sms-jes per jaar verstuurd. In Nederland en België sms’t ongeveer 80% van de jongeren regelmatig, en ongeveer 70% van hen gebruikt sms-taal (Daniels, 2009). Het gebruik van afkortingen heeft de schuld gekregen van de verloedering van de Nederlandse taal, iets dat men terug zou zien in de kwaliteit van het Nederlands in e-mails en brieven. Verder gebruik van afkortingen zou de communicatie verslechteren tussen de gebruikers van afkortingen en niet gebruikers, bijvoorbeeld tussen studenten en docenten. De vraag is of dat echt zo is. Uit mijn onderzoek blijkt dat het begrijpen van een tekst niet leidt onder het gebruik van afkortingen in de zinnen
  • Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Processing of SMS shortcuts [Keynote lecture]. Talk presented at 20th Anéla Juniorendag. Leiden, The Netherlands. 2010-01-29.
  • Meyer, A. S., Chu, M., & Ganushchak, L. (2010). Overt and masked semantic and phonological priming of picture naming. Talk presented at Competition Effects in Language Production, Workshop. Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
  • Mortensen, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Naming associated objects: Evidence for parallel processing. Talk presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010. Manchester, UK. 2010-07-07 - 2010-07-09.

    Abstract

    Earlier research has shown that speakers naming object pairs can retrieve their names in parallel, but often fail to do so. The conditions for the occurrence of parallel object processing are largely unknown. We examined how associations between objects affected the speakers’ processing strategies. In Experiment 1, participants named object triplets in a left-right-bottom order. During the saccade towards the right object (interloper), it was replaced by a new object (target). We varied the relationship between interloper and target (conceptually related or unrelated) and between interloper and left object (categorically related, associated, unrelated). Target gaze durations were shorter after related than unrelated interlopers. However, this preview effect was independent of the interloper-left object relationship, suggesting that the left and right object were processed sequentially. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants named pairs of associated or unrelated objects. The left objects were repeated several times in each test block to facilitate their processing. Gaze durations for the left objects were longer in associated than unrelated pairs, consistent with parallel processing of the associated objects and interference among them. We will discuss how the difficulty of processing the objects and the relationship between them jointly determine speakers’ processing strategy.
  • Rommers, J., Huettig, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Task-dependency in the activation of visual representations during language comprehension. Poster presented at The Embodied Mind: Perspectives and Limitations, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
  • Rommers, J., Huettig, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Task-dependent activation of visual representations during language comprehension. Poster presented at The 16th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2010], York, UK.
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Naming speed is negatively related to working memory capacity. Poster presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010, Manchester, UK.

    Abstract

    Individuals differ greatly on how efficiently they can retrieve words from the mental lexicon in spoken word production. Little is known about how this individual difference might be related to individual difference in other cognitive abilities. The present study investigated the relationship between individuals’ working memory capacity and their efficiency of lexical access. Speeded naming of drawings of actions and objects was used to measure the efficiency of lexical access. To assess working memory capacity, we used the Operation Span (OSPAN) task (Turner & Engle, 1989), in which participants memorize unrelated words while making judgments on simple mathematic operations. We found that action naming response time (RT) was significantly longer than object naming RT. More importantly, there was a significant negative correlation between the action naming RT and the OSPAN score. There was no correlation between object naming RT and the OSPAN score. Ex-Gaussian analyses of the RT distributions revealed that the negative relationship between OSPAN and RT was evident throughout the distributions (i.e., reflected in the Gaussian part of the distribution). These results suggest that when words are difficult to select (our verbcondition) people with larger working memory capacity outperform people with lower working memory capacity. Turner, M. L., & Engle. R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127-154.

Share this page