Displaying 1 - 38 of 38
-
Araújo, S., Konopka, A. E., Meyer, A. S., Hagoort, P., & Weber, K. (2018). Effects of verb position on sentence planning. Poster presented at the International Workshop on Language Production (IWLP 2018), Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Fairs, A., Bögels, S., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). Serial or parallel dual-task language processing: Production planning and comprehension are not carried out in parallel. Poster presented at the International Workshop on Language Production (IWLP 2018), Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Favier, S., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2018). Does literacy predict individual differences in the syntactic processing of spoken language?. Poster presented at the 1st Workshop on Cognitive Science of Culture, Lisbon, Portugal.
-
Favier, S., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2018). Does reading ability predict individual differences in spoken language syntactic processing?. Poster presented at the International Meeting of the Psychonomics Society 2018, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
-
Favier, S., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2018). How does literacy influence syntactic processing in spoken language?. Talk presented at Psycholinguistics in Flanders (PiF 2018). Gent, Belgium. 2018-06-04 - 2018-06-05.
-
Hintz, F., Jongman, S. R., McQueen, J. M., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). Individual differences in word production: Evidence from students with diverse educational backgrounds. Poster presented at the International Workshop on Language Production (IWLP 2018), Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Hintz, F., Jongman, S. R., Dijkhuis, M., Van 't Hoff, V., Damian, M., Schröder, S., Brysbaert, M., McQueen, J. M., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). STAIRS4WORDS: A new adaptive test for assessing receptive vocabulary size in English, Dutch, and German. Poster presented at Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2018), Berlin, Germany.
-
Hintz, F., Jongman, S. R., McQueen, J. M., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). Verbal and non-verbal predictors of word comprehension and word production. Poster presented at Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2018), Berlin, Germany.
-
Iacozza, S., Meyer, A. S., & Lev-Ari, S. (2018). Evidence for in-group biases in source memory for newly learned words. Poster presented at the International Conference on Learning and Memory (LearnMem 2018), Huntington Beach, CA, USA.
-
Jongman, S. R., Piai, V., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). Withholding speech: Does the EEG signal reflect planning for production or attention?. Poster presented at the 31st Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Davis, CA, USA.
-
Mainz, N., Smith, A. C., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). Individual differences in word learning - An exploratory study of adult native speakers. Talk presented at the Experimental Psychology Society London Meeting. London, UK. 2018-01-03 - 2018-01-05.
-
Maslowski, M., Meyer, A. S., & Bosker, H. R. (2018). Do effects of habitual speech rate normalization on perception extend to self?. Talk presented at Psycholinguistics in Flanders (PiF 2018). Ghent, Belgium. 2018-06-04 - 2018-06-05.
Abstract
Listeners are known to use contextual speech rate in processing temporally ambiguous speech sounds. For instance, a fast adjacent speech context makes a vowel sound relatively long, whereas a slow context makes it sound relatively short (Reinisch & Sjerps, 2013). Besides the local contextual speech rate, listeners also track talker-specific habitual speech rates (Reinisch, 2016; Maslowski et al., in press). However, effects of one’s own speech rate on the perception of another talker’s speech are yet unexplored. Such effects are potentially important, given that, in dialogue, a listener’s own speech often constitutes the context for the interlocutor’s speech. Three experiments tested the contribution of self-produced speech on perception of the habitual speech rate of another talker. In Experiment 1, one group of participants was instructed to speak fast (high-rate group), whereas another group had to speak slowly (low-rate group; 16 participants per group). The two groups were compared on their perception of ambiguous Dutch /A/-/a:/ vowels embedded in neutral rate speech from another talker. In Experiment 2, the same participants listened to playback of their own speech, whilst evaluating target vowels in neutral rate speech as before. Neither of these experiments provided support for the involvement of self-produced speech in perception of another talker's speech rate. Experiment 3 repeated Experiment 2 with a new participant sample, who did not know the participants from the previous two experiments. Here, a group effect was found on perception of the neutral rate talker. This result replicates the finding of Maslowski et al. that habitual speech rates are perceived relative to each other (i.e., neutral rate sounds fast in the presence of a slower talker and vice versa), with naturally produced speech. Taken together, the findings show that self-produced speech is processed differently from speech produced by others. They carry implications for our understanding of the perceptual and cognitive mechanisms involved in rate-dependent speech perception and the link between production and perception in dialogue settings. -
Maslowski, M., Meyer, A. S., & Bosker, H. R. (2018). How speech rate normalization affects lexical access. Talk presented at Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2018). Berlin, Germany. 2018-09-06 - 2018-09-08.
-
Maslowski, M., Meyer, A. S., & Bosker, H. R. (2018). Self-produced speech rate is processed differently from other talkers' rates. Poster presented at the International Workshop on Language Production (IWLP 2018), Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
Abstract
Interlocutors perceive phonemic category boundaries relative to talkers’ produced speech rates. For instance, a temporally ambiguous vowel between Dutch short /A/ and long /a:/ sounds short (i.e., as /A/) in a slow speech context, but long in a fast context. Besides the local contextual speech rate, listeners also track talker-specific habitual speech rates (Maslowski et al., in press). However, it is yet unclear whether self-produced speech rate modulates perception of another talker’s habitual rate. Such effects are potentially important, given that, in dialogue, a listener’s own speech often constitutes the context for the interlocutor’s speech. Three experiments addressed this question. In Experiment 1, one group of participants was instructed to speak fast, whereas another group had to speak slowly (16 participants per group). The two groups were then compared on their perception of ambiguous Dutch /A/-/a:/ vowels embedded in neutral rate speech from another talker. In Experiment 2, the same participants listened to playback of their own speech, whilst evaluating target vowels in neutral rate speech as before. Neither of these experiments provided support for the involvement of self-produced speech in perception of another talker's speech rate. Experiment 3 repeated Experiment 2 with a new participant sample, who were unfamiliar with the participants from the previous two experiments. Here, a group effect was found on perception of the neutral rate talker. This result replicates the finding of Maslowski et al. that habitual speech rates are perceived relative to each other (i.e., neutral rate sounds fast in the presence of a slower talker and vice versa), with naturally produced speech. Taken together, the findings show that self-produced speech is processed differently from speech produced by others. They carry implications for our understanding of the link between production and perception in dialogue. -
Raviv, L., Meyer, A. S., & Lev-Ari, S. (2018). Network structure and the cultural evolution of linguistic structure: An artificial language study. Talk presented at the Cultural Evolution Society Conference (CES 2018). Tempe, AZ, USA. 2018-10-22 - 2018-10-24.
-
Raviv, L., Meyer, A. S., & Lev-Ari, S. (2018). Social structure affects the emergence of linguistic structure: Experimental evidence. Talk presented at the Linguistics Department Colloquium Series. University of Arizona, Tuscon, AZ, USA. 2018-10-26.
-
Raviv, L., Meyer, A. S., & Lev-Ari, S. (2018). Social structure affects the emergence of linguistic structure: Experimental evidence. Talk presented at the Language evolution seminar, Center for Language evolution, University of Edinburgh. Edinburgh, UK. 2018-08-21.
-
Raviv, L., Meyer, A. S., & Lev-Ari, S. (2018). Social structure affects the emergence of linguistic structure: Experimental evidence. Talk presented at the Cohn Institute for the History and Philosophy of Science, Tel Aviv University. Tel Aviv, Israel. 2018-12-23.
-
Raviv, L., Meyer, A. S., & Lev-Ari, S. (2018). Social structure affects the emergence of linguistic structure: Experimental evidence. Talk presented at the Department of Linguistics, Tel Aviv University. Tel Aviv, Israel. 2018-12-25.
-
Raviv, L., Meyer, A. S., & Lev-Ari, S. (2018). Social structure affects the emergence of linguistic structure: Experimental evidence. Talk presented at the Donders Discussions 2018. Nijmegen, The Netherlands. 2018-10-11.
-
Raviv, L., Meyer, A. S., & Lev-Ari, S. (2018). The role of community size in the emergence of linguistic structure. Talk presented at the 12th International Conference on the Evolution of Language: (EVOLANG XII). Torun, Poland. 2018-04-15 - 2018-04-19.
-
Rodd, J., Bosker, H. R., Meyer, A. S., Ernestus, M., & Ten Bosch, L. (2018). How to speed up and slow down: Speaking rate control to the level of the syllable. Talk presented at the New Observations in Speech and Hearing seminar series, Institute of Phonetics and Speech processing, LMU Munich. Munich, Germany.
-
Rodd, J., Bosker, H. R., Ernestus, M., Meyer, A. S., & Ten Bosch, L. (2018). Run-speaking? Simulations of rate control in speech production. Poster presented at Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2018), Berlin, Germany.
-
Rodd, J., Bosker, H. R., Ernestus, M., Meyer, A. S., & Ten Bosch, L. (2018). Running or speed-walking? Simulations of speech production at different rates. Poster presented at the International Workshop on Language Production (IWLP 2018), Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
Abstract
That speakers can vary their speaking rate is evident, but how they accomplish this has
hardly been studied. The effortful experience of deviating from one's preferred speaking rate
might result from shifting between different regimes (system configurations) of the speech
planning system. This study investigates control over speech rate through simulations of a
new connectionist computational model of the cognitive process of speech production, derived
from Dell, Burger and Svec’s (1997) model to fit the temporal characteristics of observed
speech. We draw an analogy from human movement: the selection of walking and running
gaits to achieve different movement speeds. Are the regimes of the speech production system
arranged into multiple ‘gaits’ that resemble walking and running?
During training of the model, different parameter settings are identified for different speech
rates, which can be conflated with the regimes of the speech production system. The
parameters can be considered to be dimensions of a high-dimensional ‘regime space’, in
which different regimes occupy different parts of the space.
In a single gait system, the regimes are qualitatively similar, but quantitatively different.
They are arranged along a straight line through regime space. Different points along this axis
correspond directly to different speaking rates. In a multiple gait system, the arrangement of
the regimes is more disperse, with no obvious relationship between the regions associated
with each gait.
After training, the model achieved good fits in all three speaking rates, and the parameter
settings associated with each speaking rate were different. The broad arrangement of the
parameter settings for the different speaking rates in regime space was non-axial, suggesting
that ‘gaits’ may be present in the speech planning system. -
Rodd, J., Bosker, H. R., Ernestus, M., Ten Bosch, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). To speed up, turn up the gain: Acoustic evidence of a 'gain-strategy' for speech planning in accelerated and decelerated speech. Poster presented at LabPhon16 - Variation, development and impairment: Between phonetics and phonology, Lisbon, Portugal.
-
Takashima, A., Meyer, A. S., Hagoort, P., & Weber, K. (2018). Lexical and syntactic memory representations for sentence production: Effects of lexicality and verb arguments. Poster presented at the International Workshop on Language Production (IWLP 2018), Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Takashima, A., Meyer, A. S., Hagoort, P., & Weber, K. (2018). Producing sentences in the MRI scanner: Effects of lexicality and verb arguments. Poster presented at the Tenth Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language (SNL 2018), Quebec, Canada.
-
Taschenberger, L., Brehm, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). Interference in joint picture naming. Poster presented at the IMPRS Conference on Interdisciplinary Approaches in the Language Sciences, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
Abstract
In recent years, the theory that prediction is an important part of language processing has gained considerable attention (see Huettig, 2015, for overview). There is a large body of empirical evidence which suggests that language users’ ability to anticipate interlocutors’ upcoming utterances is one of the reasons why interactive speech can be so effortless, smooth, and efficient in nature (e.g. Wicha et al., 2004; van Berkum et al., 2005). The present study aimed to investigate whether the language production module is affected by prediction of another individual’s utterances using a joint language production task designed to establish whether simulation of an interlocutor’s utterance occurs automatically, even if this hinders one’s own speech production. The experiment aimed to replicate the finding of an interference effect in a joint naming task (Gambi et al., 2015), and investigate whether the same patterns could be found within a clearer social context where a partner was co-present in the same room. Participants named pictures of objects while their partners concurrently named or categorised congruent or incongruent stimuli. Analyses of naming onset latencies indicate that individuals may partially co-represent their partner’s utterances and that this shared representation influences language production. Congruency in task and stimuli display facilitated naming compared to incongruent trials which showed a tendency to impede production latencies. This finding of a social effect in a setting where simulation of language content is not necessary may suggest that some kind of anticipatory processing is an underlying feature of comprehension. -
Zormpa, E., Hoedemaker, R. S., Brehm, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2018). The production and generation effect in picture naming: How lexical access and articulation influence memory. Poster presented at the Experimental Psychology Society London Meeting, London, UK.
Abstract
Previous work on memory phenomena shows that pictures and words lead to a production effect, i.e. better memory for aloud than silent items, and that this interacts with the picture superiority effect, i.e. better memory for pictures than words (Fawcett, Quinlan and Taylor, 2012). We investigated the role of the generation effect, i.e. improved memory for generated words, in picture naming. As picture naming requires participants to think of an appropriate label, a generation effect might be elicited for pictures but not words. Forty-two participants named pictures silently or aloud and were given the correct picture name or an unreadable label; all conditions included pictures to control for the picture superiority effect. Memory was then tested using a yes/no recognition task. We found a production effect (p < 0.001) showing the role of articulation in memory, a generation effect (p < 0.001) showing the role of lexical access in memory, and an interaction (p <0.05) between the two suggesting the non-independence of the effects. Ongoing work further tests the role of label reliability in eliciting these effects. This research demonstrates a role for the generation effect in picture naming, with implications for memory asymmetries at different stages in language production.Additional information
link to poster on figshare -
Chu, M., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Name-picture verification as a control measure for object naming: Data from British English speakers. Poster presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010, Manchester, UK.
Abstract
The name-picture verification task is often used to assess the difficulty of prelexical processes (object recognition and semantic access) during picture naming. However, whether to use responses from word-picture match or from mismatch trials to index the difficulty of pre-lexical processes is debated. Levelt (2002) argued for the use of mismatch trials because on match trials the printed object name might facilitate picture recognition. However, in a study with speakers of Spanish Stadthagen-Gonzalez et al. (2009) showed that visual and conceptual properties of objects only correlated with the latencies of match responses but not with those of mismatch responses and therefore advocated the use of match responses. The present study aimed to replicate Stadthagen- Gonzalez et al. (2009) findings using native British English speakers and English norms for non-lexical and lexical variables. We replicated the finding that non-lexical variables affected the speed of match, but not mismatch responses. However, in addition, we found that lexical variables also affected the speed of match responses, which means that these latencies need to be interpreted with caution. In other words, neither match nor mismatch responses seem ideally suited to assess the difficulty of pre-lexical processes in picture naming. Levelt, W. J. M. (2002). Picture naming and word frequency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 663–671. Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., Damian, M. F., Pérez, M. A., Bowers, J. S., & Marín, J. (2009). Name-picture verification as a control measure for object naming: A task analysis and norms for a -
Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Lexical representations of SMS shortcuts. Talk presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010. Manchester, UK. 2010-07-07 - 2010-07-09.
Abstract
As the popularity of sending messages electronically increased, so did the necessity of conveying messages more efficiently. A popular way of increasing efficiency is to use shortcuts such as gr8 “great” or cu “see you”. The aim of the study was to investigate whether such abbreviations have their own lexical representations or whether they are recorded into the words they stand for. We used associative masked and overt priming in lexical decision tasks. Primes were text shortcuts and their corresponding words. Targets were words that were associatively related to the prime (e.g., cu/see you – GOODBYE), associatively related to a part of the prime (e.g., cu/see you – LOOK) or unrelated to the prime and any part of the prime (e.g., 4u/for you – GOODBYE; 4u/for you – LOOK). In both tasks, responses were faster to targets preceded by related compared to unrelated primes (shortcuts and words). In the overt priming task, we in addition found faster responses when the target was related to a part of the prime. However, this effect was present only for word but not for shortcut primes. These results indicate that shortcuts have their own lexical representations that are independent of the representations of the individual words they replace. -
Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Ondrzk vd nwe taal. Talk presented at NWO Conference Bessensap 2010. The Hague, The Netherlands. 2010-06-07.
Abstract
Ik laat zien dat het gebruik van sms- en msn-afkortingen geen schadelijke gevolgen heeft op het begrip van geschreven taal zoals blijkt uit een reeks van experimenten. Sterker nog, afkortingen hebben een eigen representatie in ons mentale woordenboek (bijvoorbeeld ‘iig’) en worden niet vertaald naar woorden waaruit ze bestaan (bijvoorbeeld ‘in ieder geval’). Het gebruik van korte tekstberichten via mobiele telefoons en e-mail neemt steeds meer toe en daardoor worden steeds meer afkortingen en acroniemen in de taal gebruikt. Momenteel worden er wereldwijd naar schatting 2000 miljard sms-jes per jaar verstuurd. In Nederland en België sms’t ongeveer 80% van de jongeren regelmatig, en ongeveer 70% van hen gebruikt sms-taal (Daniels, 2009). Het gebruik van afkortingen heeft de schuld gekregen van de verloedering van de Nederlandse taal, iets dat men terug zou zien in de kwaliteit van het Nederlands in e-mails en brieven. Verder gebruik van afkortingen zou de communicatie verslechteren tussen de gebruikers van afkortingen en niet gebruikers, bijvoorbeeld tussen studenten en docenten. De vraag is of dat echt zo is. Uit mijn onderzoek blijkt dat het begrijpen van een tekst niet leidt onder het gebruik van afkortingen in de zinnen -
Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Processing of SMS shortcuts [Keynote lecture]. Talk presented at 20th Anéla Juniorendag. Leiden, The Netherlands. 2010-01-29.
-
Meyer, A. S., Chu, M., & Ganushchak, L. (2010). Overt and masked semantic and phonological priming of picture naming. Talk presented at Competition Effects in Language Production, Workshop. Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Mortensen, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Naming associated objects: Evidence for parallel processing. Talk presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010. Manchester, UK. 2010-07-07 - 2010-07-09.
Abstract
Earlier research has shown that speakers naming object pairs can retrieve their names in parallel, but often fail to do so. The conditions for the occurrence of parallel object processing are largely unknown. We examined how associations between objects affected the speakers’ processing strategies. In Experiment 1, participants named object triplets in a left-right-bottom order. During the saccade towards the right object (interloper), it was replaced by a new object (target). We varied the relationship between interloper and target (conceptually related or unrelated) and between interloper and left object (categorically related, associated, unrelated). Target gaze durations were shorter after related than unrelated interlopers. However, this preview effect was independent of the interloper-left object relationship, suggesting that the left and right object were processed sequentially. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants named pairs of associated or unrelated objects. The left objects were repeated several times in each test block to facilitate their processing. Gaze durations for the left objects were longer in associated than unrelated pairs, consistent with parallel processing of the associated objects and interference among them. We will discuss how the difficulty of processing the objects and the relationship between them jointly determine speakers’ processing strategy. -
Rommers, J., Huettig, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Task-dependency in the activation of visual representations during language comprehension. Poster presented at The Embodied Mind: Perspectives and Limitations, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Rommers, J., Huettig, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Task-dependent activation of visual representations during language comprehension. Poster presented at The 16th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2010], York, UK.
-
Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Naming speed is negatively related to working memory capacity. Poster presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010, Manchester, UK.
Abstract
Individuals differ greatly on how efficiently they can retrieve words from the mental lexicon in spoken word production. Little is known about how this individual difference might be related to individual difference in other cognitive abilities. The present study investigated the relationship between individuals’ working memory capacity and their efficiency of lexical access. Speeded naming of drawings of actions and objects was used to measure the efficiency of lexical access. To assess working memory capacity, we used the Operation Span (OSPAN) task (Turner & Engle, 1989), in which participants memorize unrelated words while making judgments on simple mathematic operations. We found that action naming response time (RT) was significantly longer than object naming RT. More importantly, there was a significant negative correlation between the action naming RT and the OSPAN score. There was no correlation between object naming RT and the OSPAN score. Ex-Gaussian analyses of the RT distributions revealed that the negative relationship between OSPAN and RT was evident throughout the distributions (i.e., reflected in the Gaussian part of the distribution). These results suggest that when words are difficult to select (our verbcondition) people with larger working memory capacity outperform people with lower working memory capacity. Turner, M. L., & Engle. R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127-154.
Share this page