Antje Meyer

Presentations

Displaying 1 - 35 of 35
  • Acheson, D. J., Veenstra, A., Meyer, A. S., & Hagoort, P. (2014). EEG pattern classification of semantic and syntactic Influences on subject-verb agreement in production. Poster presented at the Sixth Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language (SNL 2014), Amsterdam.

    Abstract

    Subject-verb agreement is one of the most common
    grammatical encoding operations in language
    production. In many languages, morphological
    inflection on verbs code for the number of the head
    noun of a subject phrase (e.g., The key to the cabinets
    is rusty). Despite the relative ease with which subjectverb
    agreement is accomplished, people sometimes
    make agreement errors (e.g., The key to the cabinets
    are rusty). Such errors offer a window into the early
    stages of production planning. Agreement errors are
    influenced by both syntactic and semantic factors, and
    are more likely to occur when a sentence contains either
    conceptual or syntactic number mismatches. Little
    is known about the timecourse of these influences,
    however, and some controversy exists as to whether
    they are independent. The current study was designed
    to address these two issues using EEG. Semantic and
    syntactic factors influencing number mismatch were
    factorially-manipulated in a forced-choice sentence
    completion paradigm. To avoid EEG artifact associated
    with speaking, participants (N=20) were presented with
    a noun-phrase, and pressed a button to indicate which
    version of the verb ‘to be’ (is/are) should continue
    the sentence. Semantic number was manipulated
    using preambles that were semantically-integrated or
    unintegrated. Semantic integration refers to the semantic
    relationship between nouns in a noun-phrase, with
    integrated items promoting conceptual-singularity.
    The syntactic manipulation was the number (singular/
    plural) of the local noun preceding the decision. This
    led to preambles such as “The pizza with the yummy
    topping(s)... “ (integated) vs. “The pizza with the tasty
    bevarage(s)...” (unintegrated). Behavioral results showed
    effects of both Local Noun Number and Semantic
    Integration, with more errors and longer reaction times
    occurring in the mismatching conditions (i.e., plural
    local nouns; unintegrated subject phrases). Classic ERP
    analyses locked to the local noun (0-700 ms) and to the
    time preceding the response (-600 to 0 ms) showed no
    systematic differences between conditions. Despite this
    result, we assessed whether difference might emerge
    using multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA). Using the
    same epochs as above, support-vector machines with a
    radial basis function were trained on the single-trial level
    to classify the difference between Local Noun Number
    and Semantic Integration conditions across time and
    channels. Results revealed that both conditions could
    be reliably classified at the single subject level, and
    that classification accuracy was strongest in the epoch
    preceding the response. Classification accuracy was
    at chance when a classifier trained to dissociate Local
    Noun Number was used to predict Semantic Integration
    (and vice versa), providing some evidence of the
    independence of the two effects. Significant inter-subject
    variability was present in the channels and time-points
    that were critical for classification, but earlier timepoints
    were more often important for classifying Local Noun
    Number than Semantic Integration. One result of this
    variability is classification performed across subjects was
    at chance, which may explain the failure to find standard
    ERP effects. This study thus provides an important first
    test of semantic and syntactic influences on subject-verb
    agreement with EEG, and demonstrates that where
    classic ERP analyses fail, MVPA can reliably distinguish
    differences at the neurophysiological level.
  • Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2014). Mechanisms underlying predictive language processing. Talk presented at the 56. Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen [TeaP, Conference on Experimental Psychology]. Giessen, Germany. 2014-03-31 - 2014-04-02.
  • Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2014). Prediction using production or production engaging prediction?. Poster presented at the 20th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLAP 2014), Edinburgh (UK).

    Abstract

    Prominent theories of predictive language processing assume that language production processes are used to anticipate upcoming linguistic input during comprehension (Dell & Chang, 2014; Pickering & Garrod, 2013). Here, we explore the converse case: Does a task set including production in addition to comprehension encourage prediction, compared to a task only including comprehension? To test this hypothesis, we conducted a cross-modal naming experiment (Experiment 1) including an object naming task and a self-paced reading experiment (Experiment 2) that did not include overt production. We used the same predictable (N = 40) and non-predictable (N = 40) sentences in both experiments. The sentences consisted of a fixed agent, a transitive verb and a predictable or non-predictable target word (The man drinks a beer vs. The man buys a beer). Most of the empirical work on prediction used sentences in which the target words were highly predictable (often with a mean cloze probability > .8) and thus it is little surprising that participants engaged in predictive language processing very easily. In the current sentences, the mean cloze probability in the predictable sentences was .39 (ranging from .06 to .8; zero in the non-predictable sentences). If comprehenders are more likely to engage in predictive processing when the task set involves production, we should observe more pronounced effects of prediction in Experiment 1 as compared to Experiment 2. If production does not enhance prediction, we should observe similar effects of prediction in both experiments. In Experiment 1, participants (N = 54) listened to recordings of the sentences which ended right before the spoken target word. Coinciding with the end of the playback, a picture of the target word was shown which the participants were asked to name as fast as possible. Analyses of their naming latencies revealed a statistically significant naming advantage of 106 ms on predictable over non-predictable trials. Moreover, we found that the objects’ naming advantage was predicted by the target words’ cloze probability in the sentences (r = .411, p = .016). In Experiment 2, the same sentences were used in a self-paced reading experiment. To allow for testing of potential spill-over effects, we added a neutral prepositional phrase (buys a beer from the bar keeper/drinks a beer from the shop) to each sentence. Participants (N = 54) read the sentences word-by-word, advancing by pushing the space bar. On 30% of the trials, comprehension questions were used to keep up participants' focus on comprehending the sentences. Analyses of participants’ target and post-target reading times revealed numerical advantages of 6 ms and 20 ms, respectively, in the predictable as compared to the non-predictable condition. However, in both cases, this difference was not statistically reliable (t = .757, t = 1.43) and the significant positive correlation between an item’s naming advantage and its cloze probability as seen in Experiment 1 was absent (r = .037, p = .822). Importantly, the analysis of participants' responses to the comprehension questions, showed that they understood the sentences (mean accuracy = 93%). To conclude, although both experiments used the same sentences, we observed effects of prediction only when the task included production. In Experiment 2, no evidence for anticipation was found although participants clearly understood the sentences and the method has previously been shown to be sensitive to measure prediction effects (Van Berkum et al., 2005). Our results fit with a recent study by Gollan et al. (2011) who found only a small processing advantage of predictive over non-predictive sentences in reading (using highly predictable sentences with a cloze probability > . 87) but a strong prediction effect when participants read the same sentences and carried out an additional object naming task (see also Griffin & Bock, 1998). Taken together, the studies suggest that the comprehenders' task set exerts a powerful influence on the likelihood and magnitude of predictive language processing. When the task set involves language production, as is often the case in natural conversation, comprehenders might engage in prediction to a stronger degree than in pure comprehension tasks. Being able to predict words another person is about to say might optimize the comprehension process and enable smooth turn-taking.
  • Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2014). The influence of verb-specific featural restrictions, word associations, and production-based mechanisms on language-mediated anticipatory eye movements. Talk presented at the 27th annual CUNY conference on human sentence processing. Ohio State University, Columbus/Ohio (US). 2014-03-13 - 2014-03-15.
  • Jongman, S. R., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). Sustained attention in language production: An individual differences approach. Talk presented at the Experimental Psychology Society (EPS). Kent, England. 2014-04-15 - 2014-04-17.
  • Katzberg, D., Belke, E., Wrede, B., Ernst, J., Berwe, T., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). AUDIOMAX: A software using an automatic speech recognition system for fast ans accurate temporal analyses of word onsets in spoken utterances. Poster presented at the International Workshop on Language Production 2014, Geneva.
  • Moers, C., Meyer, A. S., & Janse, E. (2014). Effects of local predictability on eye fixation behavior in silent and oral reading for younger and older adults. Poster presented at the 20th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLAP 2014), Edinburgh, UK.
  • Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). Effects of local predictability on word durations and fixation rates in younger and older adults. Talk presented at Psycholinguistics in Flanders 2014 (PiF 2014). Ostend, Belgium. 2014-05-08 - 2014-05-09.
  • Schuerman, W. L., Meyer, A. S., & McQueen, J. M. (2014). Listeners recognize others’ speech better than their own. Poster presented at the 20th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLAP 2014), Edinburgh, UK.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). Parallel planning and attraction in the production of subject-verb agreement. Poster presented at the International Workshop on Language Production 2014, Geneva.
  • Gerakaki, S., Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Planning speech affects memory of heard words. Poster presented at the 12th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference, Leuven, Belgium.
  • Hintz, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Prediction and production of simple mathematical equations. Poster presented at the 18th Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP 2013), Budapest, Hungary.

    Abstract

    An important issue in current psycholinguistics is the relationship between the production and comprehension systems. It has been argued that these systems are tightly linked, and that, in particular, listeners use the speech production system to predict upcoming content. We tested this view using a novel version of the visual world paradigm. Participants heard mathematical equations and looked at a clock face showing the numbers 1 to 12. On alternating trials they either heard a complete equation (3+8=11) or they heard the first part (3+8) and had to produce the solution (11, target hereafter) themselves. Participants were encouraged to look at the relevant numbers throughout the trial. On listening trials, the participants typically looked at the target before the onset of target name, and on speaking trials they typically looked at the target before naming it. However, the timing of the looks to the targets was slightly different, with participants looking earlier at the target when they had to speak themselves than when they listened. This suggests that predicting during listening and planning to speak are indeed very similar but not identical. The further methodological and theoretical consequences of the study will be discussed.
  • Jongman, S. R., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Sustained attention in language production: An individual differences study. Talk presented at the Nederlandse Vereniging voor Psychonomie (NVP). Egmond aan Zee, The Netherlands. 2013-12-19 - 2013-12-21.
  • Konopka, A. E., Kuchinsky, S., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Does message similarity facilitate sentence formulation?. Poster presented at the 26th CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing [CUNY 2013], Columbia, SC.

    Abstract

    The generation of an utterance begins with event apprehension and continues with sequential linguistic
    encoding of all message elements [2]. The timecourse of formulation, however, is relatively flexible and varies
    with the ease of structural encoding [3]. While previous work has shown that syntactic structure may be primed
    independently of thematic roles across sentences [1], here we tested whether exposure to conceptually similar
    events interacts with structural processes to facilitate the mapping of a message onto a sentence.
  • Konopka, A. E., Kuchinsky, S., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Does message similarity facilitate sentence formulation?. Talk presented at the 11th International Symposium of Psycholinguistics. Tenerife, Spain. 2013-03-20 - 2013-03-23.
  • Meyer, A. S., Shao, Z., Randi, M., & Roelofs, A. (2013). The role of selective inhibition in semantic interference tasks. Talk presented at Bangor Meeting of the Experimental Psychology Society. Bangor. 2013-07-03 - 2013-07-05.

    Abstract

    Recently many authors have stressed that domain-general cognitive processes may affect performance in linguistic tasks. This challenges the traditional view that speaking and listening are fairly modular processes. Going beyond this broad claim, we aim to determine exactly how domain-general processes influence linguistic processes. In the present study we examined the influence of selective inhibition (invoked to suppress responses to potent competitors to target stimuli and taking some time to build up) on performance in two classic word production tasks, the semantic blocking task (naming sets of objects that do vs. do not belong to the same semantic category) and the pictureword interference task (naming pictures accompanied by categorically related vs. unrelated words). Both tasks were completed by the same participants. Analyses of the size of the interference effects for fast and slower responses (using delta plots) and of the correlations of the effect sizes in the two tasks demonstrated that selective inhibition was recruited in both tasks. We propose that the process supported by selective inhibition is lemma selection. We discuss the implications for theories concerning the origin of the interference effects in the two paradigms and the nature of lexical selection processes.
  • Meyer, A. S. (2013). What's in it for me? What’s in it for me? Applying adult speech production models to young learners. Talk presented at a Workshop at the University of Leiden. Leiden, The Netherlands. 2013-12.
  • Moers, C., Meyer, A. S., & Janse, E. (2013). Age-related effects of low-level predictability on pronunciation variation in reading aloud for younger and old speakers. Poster presented at Aging and Speech Communication: The 5th International and Interdisciplinary Research Conference, Bloomington, United States.
  • Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Effects of transitional probabilities on word durations in read speech of younger & older speakers. Talk presented at the Centre for Language and Speech Technology colloquium, Radboud University. Nijmegen, The Netherlands. 2013-02.
  • Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Effects of word frequency and transitional probability on reading durations for older adults, younger adults (and children). Talk presented at Speech Production and Aging, graduate seminar, University of California Berkeley. Berkeley, CA, USA. 2013-10.
  • Reifegerste, J., Zwitserlood, P., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). The influence of pseudoword material on the processing of Dutch past-tense verbs. Talk presented at the 8th International Morphological Processing Conference. Cambridge, UK. 2013-06-20 - 2013-06-22.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Piai, V., & Huettig, F. (2013). Constraining the involvement of language production in comprehension: A comparison of object naming and object viewing in sentence context. Talk presented at the 19th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2013]. Marseille, France. 2013-09-02 - 2013-09-04.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Praamstra, P., & Huettig, F. (2013). Anticipating references to objects during sentence comprehension. Talk presented at the Experimental Psychology Society meeting (EPS). Bangor, UK. 2013-07-03 - 2013-07-05.
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Domain-general inhibition helps lexical selection in picture naming: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Poster presented at the 11th Symposium of Psycholinguistics, Tenerife, Spain.
  • Shao, Z., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). individual differences in verbal fluency task performance in older adults. Poster presented at Aging and Speech Communication: The 5th International and Interdisciplinary Research Conference, Bloomington, United States.
  • Shao, Z., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). What do verbal fluency tasks measure?. Poster presented at the 18th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology [ESCOP 2013], Budapest, Hungary.
  • Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). The initiation of speech planning in turn-taking. Talk presented at the 18th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP). Budapest (Hungary). 2013-08-29 - 2013-09-01.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Is it better to serve a meal than to serve a banquet? Syntactic flexibility and the effects of lexical context in language production. Talk presented at the Experimental Linguistics Talks (ELiTU). Utrecht, The Netherlands. 2013-05-06.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Is it better to serve a meal than to serve a banquet? Syntactic flexibility and the effects of lexical context in language production. Poster presented at the 12th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference [PIF 2013], Leuven, Belgium.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Structure selection during sentence production: A role for executive control?. Poster presented at the 26th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing [CUNY 2013], Columbia, SC.

    Abstract

    Multiple syntactic alternatives are often available to express one message. One of the factors driving the
    choice for a syntactic frame is verb bias. This study focuses on the role of verb bias in the process of selecting
    a syntactic frame for dative sentences. While some verbs are typically used with one structure (e.g.,
    voorleggen [submit] and the prepositional object dative in Dutch), other verbs have a weaker bias towards one
    syntactic frame (e.g., voorstellen [propose]): the latter can be used interchangeably in the prepositional object
    dative (PD) and double-object dative (DO) construction, and thus allows for some degree of syntactic flexibility
    during production. On one view, syntactic flexibility may facilitate production because it enables speakers to fill
    the post-verbal sentence slots with either a direct object or an indirect object (the incremental view), while on a
    different view, flexibility can lead to competition between structural alternatives, delaying the production of the
    sentence until this competition is resolved (the competition view)1. The two views make opposite predictions
    regarding the production of sentences featuring verbs with different biases. The incremental view predicts
    shorter verb onsets for sentences featuring weak-bias verbs than strong-bias verbs, while the competition view
    predicts shorter onsets for sentences with strong-bias verbs. In addition, if the competition view holds,
    sentence production may benefit from a mechanism that helps resolve competition between two syntactic
    frames by suppressing one frame to enable fast selection of the other frame. We hypothesized that executive
    control (EC) can mediate this selection process, facilitating structure selection in the weak verb bias condition.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Structure selection during sentence production: A role for executive control?. Poster presented at the 11th International Symposium of Psycholinguistics, Tenerife, Spain.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Conceptual and grammatical factors in the production of subject-verb agreement in Dutch. Talk presented at the meeting of the Experimental Psychology Society. London, UK. 2013-01-03 - 2013-01-04.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Production of subject-verb agreement. Talk presented at the Taalkunde in Nederland-dag (TiN-dag 2013). Utrecht, The Netherlands. 2013-02-09.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Subject-verb agreement and the role of semantic context. Talk presented at the 34th TABU Dag. Groningen, The Netherlands. 2013-06-13 - 2013-06-14.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Subject-verb agreement in Dutch and the role of semantic context. Talk presented at the 12th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference. Leuven, Belgium. 2013-05-30 - 2013-05-31.

Share this page