Displaying 1 - 35 of 35
-
Akamine, S., Dingemanse, M., Meyer, A. S., & Ozyurek, A. (2023). Contextual influences on multimodal alignment in Zoom interaction. Talk presented at the 1st International Multimodal Communication Symposium (MMSYM 2023). Barcelona, Spain. 2023-04-26 - 2023-04-28.
-
Bethke, S., Meyer, A. S., & Hintz, F. (2023). Developing the individual differences in language skills (IDLaS-DE) test battery—A new tool for German. Poster presented at Psycholinguistics in Flanders (PiF 2023), Ghent, Belgium.
-
Bujok, R., Peeters, D., Meyer, A. S., & Bosker, H. R. (2023). When the beat drops – beat gestures recalibrate lexical stress perception. Talk presented at the 1st International Multimodal Communication Symposium (MMSYM 2023). Barcelona, Spain. 2023-04-26 - 2023-04-28.
-
Bujok, R., Peeters, D., Meyer, A. S., & Bosker, H. R. (2023). Beat gestures can drive recalibration of lexical stress perception. Poster presented at the 5th Phonetics and Phonology in Europe Conference (PaPE 2023), Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Bujok, R., Peeters, D., Meyer, A. S., & Bosker, H. R. (2023). Beat gestures can drive recalibration of lexical stress perception. Poster presented at the Donders Poster Session 2023, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Chauvet, J., Slaats, S., Poeppel, D., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). The syllable frequency effect before and after speaking. Poster presented at the 15th Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language (SNL 2023), Marseille, France.
Abstract
Speaking requires translating concepts into a sequence of sounds. Contemporary models of language production assume that this translation involves a series of steps: from selecting the concepts to be expressed, to phonetic and articulatory encoding of the words. In addition, speakers monitor their planned output using sensorimotor predictive mechanisms. The current work concerns phonetic encoding and the speaker's monitoring of articulation. Specifically, we test whether monitoring is sensitive to the frequency of syllable-sized representations.
We run a series of immediate and delayed syllable production experiments (repetition and reading). We exploit the syllable-frequency effect: in immediate naming, high-frequency syllables are produced faster than low-frequency syllables. The effect is thought to reflect the stronger automatization of motor plan retrieval of high-frequency syllables during phonetic encoding. We predict distinct ERP and spatiotemporal patterns for high- vs. low-frequency syllables. Following articulation, we analyse auditory-evoked N1 responses that – among other features – reflect the suppression of one's own speech. Low-frequency syllables are expected to require more close monitoring, and therefore smaller N1/P2 amplitudes. The results can be important as effects of syllable frequency stand to inform us about the tradeoff between stored versus assembled representations for setting sensory targets in the production of speech. -
Chauvet, J., Slaats, S., Poeppel, D., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). The syllable frequency effect before and after speaking. Poster presented at the 19th NVP Winter Conference on Brain and Cognition, Egmond aan Zee, Netherlands.
Abstract
Speaking requires translating concepts into a sequence of sounds. Contemporary models of language production assume that this translation involves a series of steps: from selecting the concepts to be expressed, to phonetic and articulatory encoding of the words. In addition, speakers monitor their planned output using sensorimotor predictive mechanisms. The current work concerns phonetic encoding and the speaker's monitoring of articulation. Specifically, we test whether monitoring is sensitive to the frequency of syllable-sized representations.
We run a series of immediate and delayed syllable production experiments (repetition and reading). We exploit the syllable-frequency effect: in immediate naming, high-frequency syllables are produced faster than low-frequency syllables. The effect is thought to reflect the stronger automatization of motor plan retrieval of high-frequency syllables during phonetic encoding. We predict distinct ERP and spatiotemporal patterns for high- vs. low-frequency syllables. Following articulation, we analyse auditory-evoked N1 responses that – among other features – reflect the suppression of one's own speech. Low-frequency syllables are expected to require more close monitoring, and therefore smaller N1/P2 amplitudes. The results can be important as effects of syllable frequency stand to inform us about the tradeoff between stored versus assembled representations for setting sensory targets in the production of speech.
-
Corps, R. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). Repetition leads to long-term suppression of the word frequency effect. Talk presented at Psycholinguistics in Flanders (PiF 2023). Ghent, Belgium. 2023-05-29 - 2023-05-31.
-
Meyer, A. S., Schulz, F., & Hintz, F. (2023). Accounting for good enough conversational speech. Talk presented at the IndiPrag Workshop. Saarbruecken, Germany. 2023-09-18 - 2023-09-19.
-
Papoutsi, C., Tourtouri, E. N., Piai, V., Lampe, L. F., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). Fast and efficient or slow and struggling? Comparing the response times of errors and targets in speeded word production. Poster presented at the 29th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP 2023), Donostia–San Sebastián, Spain.
-
Schulz, F. M., Corps, R. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). Individual differences in the production of speech disfluencies. Poster presented at Psycholinguistics in Flanders (PiF 2023), Ghent, Belgium.
-
Schulz, F. M., Corps, R. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). Individual differences in the production of speech disfluencies. Poster presented at the 29th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP 2023), Donostia–San Sebastián, Spain.
-
Schulz, F. M., Corps, R. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). Individual differences in disfluency production. Poster presented at the 19th NVP Winter Conference on Brain and Cognition, Egmond aan Zee, The Netherlands.
Abstract
Producing spontaneous speech is challenging. It often contains disfluencies like repetitions, prolongations, silent pauses or filled pauses. Previous research has largely focused on the language-based factors (e.g., planning difficulties) underlying the production of these disfluencies. But research has also shown that some speakers are more disfluent than others. What cognitive mechanisms underlie this difference? We reanalyzed a behavioural dataset of 112 participants, who were assessed on a battery of tasks testing linguistic knowledge, processing speed, non-verbal IQ, working memory, and basic production skills and also produced six 1-minute samples of spontaneous speech (Hintz et al., 2020). We assessed the length and lexical diversity of participants’ speech and determined how often they produced silent pauses and filled pauses. We used network analysis, factor analysis and non-parametric regressions to investigate the relationship between these variables and individual differences in particular cognitive skills. We found that individual differences in linguistic knowledge or processing speed were not related to the production of disfluencies. In contrast, the proportion of filled pauses (relative to all words in the 1-minute narratives) correlated negatively with working memory capacity. -
Slaats, S., Meyer, A. S., & Martin, A. E. (2023). Do surprisal and entropy affect delta-band signatures of syntactic processing?. Poster presented at the 29th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP 2023), Donostia–San Sebastián, Spain.
-
Slaats, S., Meyer, A. S., & Martin, A. E. (2023). Do surprisal and entropy affect delta-band signatures of syntactic processing?. Poster presented at the 15th Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language (SNL 2023), Marseille, France.
-
Tourtouri, E. N., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). If you hear something (don’t) say something: A dual-EEG study on sentence processing in conversational settings. Poster presented at the 29th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP 2023), Donostia–San Sebastián, Spain.
-
Uluşahin, O., Bosker, H. R., McQueen, J. M., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). No evidence for convergence to sub-phonemic F2 shifts in shadowing. Poster presented at the 20th International Congress of the Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS 2023), Prague, Czech Republic.
-
Uluşahin, O., Bosker, H. R., McQueen, J. M., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). The influence of contextual and talker F0 information on fricative perception. Poster presented at the 5th Phonetics and Phonology in Europe Conference (PaPE 2023), Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Uluşahin, O., Bosker, H. R., McQueen, J. M., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). Listeners converge to fundamental frequency in synchronous speech. Poster presented at the 19th NVP Winter Conference on Brain and Cognition, Egmond aan Zee, The Netherlands.
Abstract
Convergence broadly refers to interlocutors’ tendency to progressively sound more like each other over time. Recent empirical work has used various experimental paradigms to observe convergence in voice fundamental frequency (f0). One study used stable mean f0 over trials in a synchronous speech task with manipulated (i.e., high and low) f0 conditions (Bradshaw & McGettigan, 2021). Here, we attempted to replicate this study in Dutch. First, in a reading task, participants read 40 sentences at their own pace to establish f0 baselines. Later, in a synchronous speech task, participants read 80 sentences in synchrony with a speaker whose voice was manipulated ±2st above or below (i.e., for the high and low f0 conditions, respectively) a reference mean f0 value. The reference mean f0 value and the manipulation size were obtained across multiple pre-tests. Our results revealed that the f0 manipulation significantly predicted f0 convergence in both high f0 and low f0 conditions. Furthermore, the proportion of convergers in the sample was larger than those reported by Bradshaw & McGettigan, highlighting the benefits of stimulus optimization. Our study thus provides stronger evidence that the pitch of two talkers tends to converge as they speak together. -
van der Burght, C. L., Schipperus, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). Does syntactic category constrain semantic interference during sentence production? A replication of Momma et al. (2020). Poster presented at the 29th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP 2023), Donostia–San Sebastián, Spain.
-
van der Burght, C. L., & Meyer, A. S. (2023). Does syntactic category constrain semantic interference effects during sentence production? A replication of Momma et al (2020). Poster presented at the 19th NVP Winter Conference on Brain and Cognition, Egmond aan Zee, The Netherlands.
Abstract
The semantic interference effect in picture naming entails longer naming latencies for pictures presented with semantically related versus unrelated distractors. One factor suggested to influence the effect is word category. However, results have been inconclusive. Momma et al. (2020) used a sentence-picture interference paradigm where the sentence context (“her singing” or “she’s singing”) disambiguated the word category (noun or verb, respectively) of distractor and target, manipulating their word category match/mismatch. Semantic interference was only found when distractor and target belonged to the same word category, suggesting that syntactic category constrains lexical competition during sentence production. Considering this important theoretical conclusion, we conducted a preregistered replication study with Dutch participants, mirroring the design of the original study. In each of 2 experiments, 60 native speakers read sentences containing sentence-final distractor words that had to be interpreted as nouns or verbs, depending on the sentence context. Subsequently, they named target action pictures as either verbs (experiment 1) or nouns (experiment 2). Results of Experiment 1 showed a main effect of relatedness, suggesting a semantic interference effect regardless of word category. We discuss differences between the original and current study results with cross-linguistic differences in (de)compositional processing and frequency of distractor forms. -
Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2015). Context-dependent employment of mechanisms in anticipatory language processing. Talk presented at the 15th NVP Winter Conference. Egmond aan Zee, The Netherlands. 2015-12-17 - 2015-12-19.
-
Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2015). Doing a production task encourages prediction: Evidence from interleaved object naming and sentence reading. Poster presented at the 28th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Los Angeles (CA, USA).
Abstract
Prominent theories of predictive language processing assume that language production processes are used to anticipate upcoming linguistic input during comprehension (Dell & Chang, 2014; Pickering & Garrod, 2013). Here, we explored the converse case: Does a task set including production in addition to comprehension encourage prediction, compared to a task only including comprehension? To test this hypothesis, participants carried out a cross-modal naming task (Exp 1a), a self-paced reading task (Exp1 b) that did not include overt production, and a task (Exp 1c) in which naming and reading trials were evenly interleaved. We used the same predictable (N = 40) and non-predictable (N = 40) sentences in all three tasks. The sentences consisted of a fixed agent, a transitive verb and a predictable or non-predictable target word (The man breaks a glass vs. The man borrows a glass). The mean cloze probability in the predictable sentences was .39 (ranging from .06 to .8; zero in the non-predictable sentences). A total of 162 volunteers took part in the experiment which was run in a between-participants design. In Exp 1a, fifty-four participants listened to recordings of the sentences which ended right before the spoken target word. Coinciding with the end of the playback, a picture of the target word was shown which the participants were asked to name as fast as possible. Analyses of their naming latencies revealed a statistically significant naming advantage of 108 ms on predictable over non-predictable trials. Moreover, we found that the objects’ naming advantage was predicted by the target words’ cloze probability in the sentences (r = .347, p = .038). In Exp 1b, 54 participants were asked to read the same sentences in a self-paced fashion. To allow for testing of potential spillover effects, we added a neutral prepositional phrase (breaks a glass from the collection/borrows a glass from the neighbor) to each sentence. The sentences were read word-by-word, advancing by pushing the space bar. On 30% of the trials, comprehension questions were used to keep up participants' focus on comprehending the sentences. Analyses of their spillover region reading times revealed a numerical advantage (8 ms; tspillover = -1.1, n.s.) in the predictable as compared to the non-predictable condition. Importantly, the analysis of participants' responses to the comprehension questions, showed that they understood the sentences (mean accuracy = 93%). In Exp 1c, the task comprised 50% naming trials and 50% reading trials which appeared in random order. Fifty-four participants named and read the same objects and sentences as in the previous versions. The results showed a naming advantage on predictable over non-predictable items (99 ms) and a positive correlation between the items’ cloze probability and their naming advantage (r = .322, p = .055). Crucially, the post-target reading time analysis showed that with naming trials and reading trials interleaved, there was also a statistically reliable prediction effect on reading trials. Participants were 19 ms faster at reading the spillover region on predictable relative to non-predictable items (tspillover = -2.624). To summarize, although we used the same sentences in all sub-experiments, we observed effects of prediction only when the task set involved production. In the reading only experiment (Exp 1b), no evidence for anticipation was obtained although participants clearly understood the sentences and the same sentences yielded reading facilitation when interleaved with naming trials (Exp 1c). This suggests that predictive language processing can be modulated by the comprehenders’ task set. When the task set involves language production, as is often the case in natural conversation, comprehenders appear to engage in prediction to a stronger degree than in pure comprehension tasks. In our discussion, we will discuss the notion that language production may engage prediction, because being able to predict words another person is about to say might optimize the comprehension process and enable smooth turn-taking. -
Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2015). Event knowledge and word associations jointly influence predictive processing during discourse comprehension. Poster presented at the 28th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Los Angeles (CA, USA).
Abstract
A substantial body of literature has shown that readers and listeners often anticipate information. An open question concerns the mechanisms underlying predictive language processing. Multiple mechanisms have been suggested. One proposal is that comprehenders use event knowledge to predict upcoming words. Other theoretical frameworks propose that predictions are made based on simple word associations. In a recent EEG study, Metusalem and colleagues reported evidence for the modulating influence of event knowledge on prediction. They examined the degree to which event knowledge is activated during sentence comprehension. Their participants read two sentences, establishing an event scenario, which were followed by a final sentence containing one of three target words: a highly expected word, a semantically unexpected word that was related to the described event, or a semantically unexpected and event-unrelated word (see Figure, for an example). Analyses of participants’ ERPs elicited by the target words revealed a three-way split with regard to the amplitude of the N400 elicited by the different types of target: the expected targets elicited the smallest N400, the unexpected and event-unrelated targets elicited the largest N400. Importantly, the amplitude of the N400 elicited by the unexpected but event-related targets was significantly attenuated relative to the amplitude of the N400 elicited by the unexpected and event-unrelated targets. Metusalem et al. concluded that event knowledge is immediately available to constrain on-line language processing. Based on a post-hoc analysis, the authors rejected the possibility that the results could be explained by simple word associations. In the present study, we addressed the role of simple word associations in discourse comprehension more directly. Specifically, we explored the contribution of associative priming to the graded N400 pattern seen in Metusalem et al’s study. We conducted two EEG experiments. In Experiment 1, we reran Metusalem and colleagues’ context manipulation and closely replicated their results. In Experiment 2, we selected two words from the event-establishing sentences which were most strongly associated with the unexpected but event-related targets in the final sentences. Each of the two associates was then placed in a neutral carrier sentence. We controlled that none of the other words in these carrier sentences was associatively related to the target words. Importantly, the two carrier sentences did not build up a coherent event. We recorded EEG while participants read the carrier sentences followed by the same final sentences as in Experiment 1. The results showed that as in Experiment 1 the amplitude of the N400 elicited by both types of unexpected target words was larger than the N400 elicited by the highly expected target. Moreover, we found a global tendency towards the critical difference between event-related and event-unrelated unexpected targets which reached statistical significance only at parietal electrodes over the right hemisphere. Because the difference between event-related and event-unrelated conditions was larger when the sentences formed a coherent event compared to when they did not, our results suggest that associative priming alone cannot account for the N400 pattern observed in our Experiment 1 (and in the study by Metusalem et al.). However, because part of the effect remained, probably due to associative facilitation, the findings demonstrate that during discourse reading both event knowledge activation and simple word associations jointly contribute to the prediction process. The results highlight that multiple mechanisms underlie predictive language processing. -
Jongman, S. R., Roelofs, A., Meyer, A. S., & Scheper, A. (2015). Sustained attention ability affects language production performance in typical and developmentally impaired children. Poster presented at the 19th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCoP 2015), Paphos, Cyprus.
-
Jongman, S. R., Roelofs, A., Meyer, A. S., & Scheper, A. (2015). Sustained attention ability affects language production performance in typical and developmentally impaired children. Poster presented at the 21st Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2015), Valletta (Malta).
-
Meyer, A. S., Shao, Z., & Van Paridon, J. (2015). Producing complex noun phrases: The roles of word and phrase frequency. Talk presented at the Psychonomic Society's 56th Annual Meeting. Chicago, USA. 2015-11-19 - 2015-11-22.
Abstract
Janssen and Barber (2012) reported two studies on the production of complex noun phrases (Spanish and French noun-adjective and noun-noun phrases). They found that production latencies depended only on the frequencies of the phrases, but not on the frequencies of the individual words. This pattern may be seen as evidence for lexical storage of phrases and against the traditional “words & rules” view of the representation of linguistic knowledge. We will discuss a series of experiments on the production of Dutch adjective-noun phrases. We replicated the phrase frequency effect seen by Janssen and Barber, but also found a robust effect of the frequency of the first word of the phrase. We argue that the phrase frequency effect arises during the conceptual preparation of the utterance and that the results are consistent with the view that phrases are composed by combining individual words, in line with the “words & rules” view. -
Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Probabilistic reduction in reading aloud: A comparison of younger and older adults. Poster presented at the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS 2015), Glasgow, Scotland, UK.
Abstract
Frequent and predictable words are generally pronounced with less effort and are therefore acoustically more reduced than less frequent or unpredictable words. Local predictability can be operationalised by Transitional Probability (TP), which indicates how likely a word is to occur given its immediate context. We investigated whether and how probabilistic reduction effects on word durations change with adult age when reading aloud content words embedded in sentences.
The results showed equally large frequency effects on verb and noun durations for both younger (Mage = 20 years) and older (Mage = 68 years) adults. Backward TP also affected word duration for younger and older adults alike. ForwardTP, however, had no significant effect on word duration in either age group.
Our results resemble earlier findings of more robust BackwardTP effects compared to ForwardTP effects. Furthermore, unlike often reported decline in predictive processing with aging, probabilistic reduction effects remain stable across adulthood. -
Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., & Praamstra, P. (2015). Tracking double-object naming using the N2pc. Poster presented at the Seventh Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language (SNL 2015), Chicago, IL.
-
Shao, Z., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Word and phrase frequency effects in noun phrase production. Talk presented at the Meeting at the Experimental Psychology Society 2015. London, UK. 2015-01-08.
-
Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). The initiation of speech planning in turn-taking. Poster presented at the 21st Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2015), Malta.
-
Tromp, J., Peeters, D., Hagoort, P., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Combining EEG and virtual reality: The N400 in a virtual environment. Talk presented at the 4th edition of the Donders Discussions (DD, 2015). Nijmegen, Netherlands. 2015-11-05 - 2015-11-06.
Abstract
A recurring criticism in the field of psycholinguistics and is the lack of ecological validity of experimental designs. For example, many experiments on sentence comprehension are conducted enclosed booths, where sentences are presented word by word on a computer screen. In addition, very often participants are instructed to make judgments that relate directly to the experimental manipulation. Thus, the contexts in which these processes are studied is quite restricted, which calls into question the generalizability of the results to more naturalistic environments. A possible solution to this problem is the use of virtual reality (VR) in psycholinguistic experiments. By immersing participants into a virtual environment, ecological validity can be increased while experimental control is maintained.
In the current experiment we combine electroencephalography (EEG) and VR to look at semantic processing in a more naturalistic setting. During the experiment, participants move through a visually rich virtual restaurant. Tables and avatars are placed in the restaurant and participants are instructed to stop at each table and look at the object (e.g. a plate with a steak) in front of the avatar. Then, the avatar will produce an utterance to accompany the object (e.g. “I think this steak is very nice”), in which the noun will either match (e.g. steak) or mismatch (e.g. mandarin) with the item on the table. Based on previous research, we predict a modulation of the N400, which should be larger in the mismatch than the match condition. Implications of the use of virtual reality for experimental research will be discussed. -
Tromp, J., Hagoort, P., & Meyer, A. S. (2015). Indirect request comprehension requires additional processing effort: A pupillometry study. Poster presented at the 19th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCoP 2015), Paphos, Cyprus.
-
Tromp, J., Meyer, A. S., & Hagoort, P. (2015). Pupillometry reveals increased processing demands for indirect request comprehension. Poster presented at the 14th International Pragmatics Conference, Antwerp, Belgium.
Abstract
Fluctuations in pupil size have been shown to reflect variations in processing demands during language
comprehension. Increases in pupil diameter have been observed as a consequence of syntactic anomalies
(Schluroff 1982), increased syntactic complexity (Just & Carpenter 1993) and lexical ambiguity (Ben-
Nun 1986). An issue that has not received attention is whether pupil size also varies due to pragmatic
manipulations. In a pupillometry experiment, we investigated whether pupil diameter is sensitive to
increased processing demands as a result of comprehending an indirect request versus a statement. During
natural conversation, communication is often indirect. For example, in an appropriate context, ''It'' cold in
here'' is a request to shut the window, rather than a statement about room temperature (Holtgraves 1994).
We tested 49 Dutch participants (mean age = 20.8). They were presented with 120 picture-sentence
combinations that could either be interpreted as an indirect request (a picture of a window with the
sentence ''it's hot here'') or as a statement (a picture of a window with the sentence ''it's nice here''). The
indirect requests were non-conventional, i.e. they did not contain directive propositional content and were
not directly related to the underlying felicity conditions (Holtgraves 2002). In order to verify that the
indirect requests were recognized, participants were asked to decide after each combination whether or
not they heard a request. Based on the hypothesis that understanding this type of indirect utterances
requires additional inferences to be made on the part of the listener (e.g., Holtgraves 2002; Searle 1975;
Van Ackeren et al. 2012), we predicted a larger pupil diameter for indirect requests than statements. The
data were analyzed using linear mixed-effects models in R, which allow for simultaneous inclusion of
participants and items as random factors (Baayen, Davidson, & Bates 2008). The results revealed a larger
mean pupil size and a larger peak pupil size for indirect requests as compared to statements. In line with
previous studies on pupil size and language comprehension (e.g., Just & Carpenter 1993), this difference
was observed within a 1.5 second window after critical word onset. We suggest that the increase in pupil
size reflects additional on-line processing demands for the comprehension of non-conventional indirect
requests as compared to statements. This supports the idea that comprehending this type of indirect
request requires capacity demanding inferencing on the part of the listener. In addition, this study
demonstrates the usefulness of pupillometry as a tool for experimental research in pragmatics. -
Tromp, J., Meyer, A. S., & Hagoort, P. (2015). Pupillometry reveals increased processing demands for indirect request comprehension. Poster presented at the 21st Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing (AMLaP 2015), Valetta, Malta.
Abstract
Fluctuations in pupil size have been shown to reflect variations in processing demands during language
comprehension. Increases in pupil diameter have been observed as a consequence of syntactic anomalies
(Schluroff 1982), increased syntactic complexity (Just & Carpenter 1993) and lexical ambiguity (Ben-
Nun 1986). An issue that has not received attention is whether pupil size also varies due to pragmatic
manipulations. In a pupillometry experiment, we investigated whether pupil diameter is sensitive to
increased processing demands as a result of comprehending an indirect request versus a statement. During
natural conversation, communication is often indirect. For example, in an appropriate context, ''It'' cold in
here'' is a request to shut the window, rather than a statement about room temperature (Holtgraves 1994).
We tested 49 Dutch participants (mean age = 20.8). They were presented with 120 picture-sentence
combinations that could either be interpreted as an indirect request (a picture of a window with the
sentence ''it's hot here'') or as a statement (a picture of a window with the sentence ''it's nice here''). The
indirect requests were non-conventional, i.e. they did not contain directive propositional content and were
not directly related to the underlying felicity conditions (Holtgraves 2002). In order to verify that the
indirect requests were recognized, participants were asked to decide after each combination whether or
not they heard a request. Based on the hypothesis that understanding this type of indirect utterances
requires additional inferences to be made on the part of the listener (e.g., Holtgraves 2002; Searle 1975;
Van Ackeren et al. 2012), we predicted a larger pupil diameter for indirect requests than statements. The
data were analyzed using linear mixed-effects models in R, which allow for simultaneous inclusion of
participants and items as random factors (Baayen, Davidson, & Bates 2008). The results revealed a larger
mean pupil size and a larger peak pupil size for indirect requests as compared to statements. In line with
previous studies on pupil size and language comprehension (e.g., Just & Carpenter 1993), this difference
was observed within a 1.5 second window after critical word onset. We suggest that the increase in pupil
size reflects additional on-line processing demands for the comprehension of non-conventional indirect
requests as compared to statements. This supports the idea that comprehending this type of indirect
request requires capacity demanding inferencing on the part of the listener. In addition, this study
demonstrates the usefulness of pupillometry as a tool for experimental research in pragmatics.
Share this page