Antje Meyer

Presentations

Displaying 201 - 255 of 255
  • Moers, C., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Effects of word frequency and transitional probability on reading durations for older adults, younger adults (and children). Talk presented at Speech Production and Aging, graduate seminar, University of California Berkeley. Berkeley, CA, USA. 2013-10.
  • Reifegerste, J., Zwitserlood, P., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). The influence of pseudoword material on the processing of Dutch past-tense verbs. Talk presented at the 8th International Morphological Processing Conference. Cambridge, UK. 2013-06-20 - 2013-06-22.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Piai, V., & Huettig, F. (2013). Constraining the involvement of language production in comprehension: A comparison of object naming and object viewing in sentence context. Talk presented at the 19th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2013]. Marseille, France. 2013-09-02 - 2013-09-04.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Praamstra, P., & Huettig, F. (2013). Anticipating references to objects during sentence comprehension. Talk presented at the Experimental Psychology Society meeting (EPS). Bangor, UK. 2013-07-03 - 2013-07-05.
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Domain-general inhibition helps lexical selection in picture naming: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Poster presented at the 11th Symposium of Psycholinguistics, Tenerife, Spain.
  • Shao, Z., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). individual differences in verbal fluency task performance in older adults. Poster presented at Aging and Speech Communication: The 5th International and Interdisciplinary Research Conference, Bloomington, United States.
  • Shao, Z., Janse, E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). What do verbal fluency tasks measure?. Poster presented at the 18th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology [ESCOP 2013], Budapest, Hungary.
  • Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). The initiation of speech planning in turn-taking. Talk presented at the 18th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP). Budapest (Hungary). 2013-08-29 - 2013-09-01.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Is it better to serve a meal than to serve a banquet? Syntactic flexibility and the effects of lexical context in language production. Talk presented at the Experimental Linguistics Talks (ELiTU). Utrecht, The Netherlands. 2013-05-06.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Is it better to serve a meal than to serve a banquet? Syntactic flexibility and the effects of lexical context in language production. Poster presented at the 12th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference [PIF 2013], Leuven, Belgium.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Structure selection during sentence production: A role for executive control?. Poster presented at the 26th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing [CUNY 2013], Columbia, SC.

    Abstract

    Multiple syntactic alternatives are often available to express one message. One of the factors driving the
    choice for a syntactic frame is verb bias. This study focuses on the role of verb bias in the process of selecting
    a syntactic frame for dative sentences. While some verbs are typically used with one structure (e.g.,
    voorleggen [submit] and the prepositional object dative in Dutch), other verbs have a weaker bias towards one
    syntactic frame (e.g., voorstellen [propose]): the latter can be used interchangeably in the prepositional object
    dative (PD) and double-object dative (DO) construction, and thus allows for some degree of syntactic flexibility
    during production. On one view, syntactic flexibility may facilitate production because it enables speakers to fill
    the post-verbal sentence slots with either a direct object or an indirect object (the incremental view), while on a
    different view, flexibility can lead to competition between structural alternatives, delaying the production of the
    sentence until this competition is resolved (the competition view)1. The two views make opposite predictions
    regarding the production of sentences featuring verbs with different biases. The incremental view predicts
    shorter verb onsets for sentences featuring weak-bias verbs than strong-bias verbs, while the competition view
    predicts shorter onsets for sentences with strong-bias verbs. In addition, if the competition view holds,
    sentence production may benefit from a mechanism that helps resolve competition between two syntactic
    frames by suppressing one frame to enable fast selection of the other frame. We hypothesized that executive
    control (EC) can mediate this selection process, facilitating structure selection in the weak verb bias condition.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Structure selection during sentence production: A role for executive control?. Poster presented at the 11th International Symposium of Psycholinguistics, Tenerife, Spain.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Conceptual and grammatical factors in the production of subject-verb agreement in Dutch. Talk presented at the meeting of the Experimental Psychology Society. London, UK. 2013-01-03 - 2013-01-04.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Production of subject-verb agreement. Talk presented at the Taalkunde in Nederland-dag (TiN-dag 2013). Utrecht, The Netherlands. 2013-02-09.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Subject-verb agreement and the role of semantic context. Talk presented at the 34th TABU Dag. Groningen, The Netherlands. 2013-06-13 - 2013-06-14.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2013). Subject-verb agreement in Dutch and the role of semantic context. Talk presented at the 12th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference. Leuven, Belgium. 2013-05-30 - 2013-05-31.
  • Hintz, F., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2012). Looking at nothing facilitates memory retrieval. Poster presented at Donders Discussions 2012, Nijmegen (NL).

    Abstract

    When processing visual objects, we integrate visual, linguistic and spatial information to form an episodic trace. Re-activating one aspect of the episodic trace of an object re-activates the entire bundle making all integrated information available. Using the blank screen paradigm [1], researchers observed that upon processing spoken linguistic input, participants tended to make eye movements on a blank screen, fixating locations that were previously occupied by objects mentioned in the linguistic utterance or were related. Ferreira and colleagues [2] suggested that 'looking at nothing' facilitated memory retrieval. However, this claim lacks convincing empirical support. In Experiment 1, Dutch participants looked at four-object-displays. Three objects were related to a spoken target word. Given the target word 'beker' (beaker), the display featured a phonological (a bear), a shape (a bobbin), a semantic (a fork) competitor, and an unrelated distractor (an umbrella). Participants were asked to name the objects as fast as possible. Subsequently, the objects disappeared. Participants fixated the center of the screen and listened to the target word. They had to carry out a semantic judgment task (indicating in which position an object had appeared that was semantically related to the objects) or a visual shape similarity judgment (indicating the position of the object similar in shape to the target). In both conditions, we observed that participants re-fixated the empty target location before responding. The set-up of Experiment 2 was identical except that we asked participants to maintain fixating the center of the screen while listening to the spoken word and responding. Performance accuracy was significantly lower in Experiment 2 than in Experiment 1. The results indicate that memory retrieval for objects is impaired when participants are not allowed to look at relevant, though empty locations. [1] Altmann, G. (2004). Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: the 'blank screen paradigm'. Cognition, 93(2), B79-B87. [2] Ferreira, F., Apel, J., & Henderson, J. M. (2008). Taking a new look at looking at nothing. Trends Cogn Sci, 12(11), 405-410.
  • Konopka, A. E., Van de Velde, M., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Mapping “easy” and “hard” messages onto language: Conceptual and structural variables jointly affect the timecourse of sentence formulation. Poster presented at the 18th Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2012], Riva del Garda, Italy.

    Abstract

    Sentence formulation requires mapping pre-verbal messages onto linguistic structures. This message-to-language
    mapping is often evaluated in eye-tracking tasks where speakers describe pictured events (The dog chased the mailman).
    Speakers can begin sentence formulation by quickly selecting the first-fixated character as the sentential starting point
    (lexical incrementality), or generating a rudimentary sentence plan based on their construal of the event gist before
    selecting a starting point (hierarchical incrementality; Kuchinsky & Bock, 2010). Lexical incrementality predicts fast
    divergence of fixations while hierarchical incrementality predicts slower divergence of fixations to the two characters
    within 200ms of picture onset.
  • Lesage, E., Morgan, B., Olson, A., Meyer, A. S., & Miall, R. (2012). Disruption of right cerebellum with rTMS blocks predictive language processing. Poster presented at the 42nd annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience [Neuroscience 2012] Poster# 379.07/UU5, New Orleans, LA.

    Abstract

    Much evidence demonstrates cerebellar involvement in language [1] but a theoretical framework about its precise role is lacking. In cerebellar motor control an influential model ascribes the cerebellum a predictive role [2]. It has been argued that cerebellar nonmotor regions perform similar computations as motor regions, and both are involved in online prediction [2]. We test this hypothesis by administering repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to the right cerebellum, a region implicated in language [3] during a predictive language task. Methods Visual World task [4]: Participants' eye movements were recorded while they listened to sentences and looked at a computer display of an agent and 4 objects, one of which (the target) was mentioned in the sentence. In the Prediction condition the object could be predicted on the basis of the verb; on Control trials it could not. We hypothesised that rTMS to the right cerebellum should make target fixation slower in the Prediction condition, but not in the Control condition. TMS protocol: TMS was delivered between two task blocks. In the cerebellar rTMS group (n = 22) the stimulation site was 1cm down and 3cm right of the inion. Participants received 10min of 1Hz rTMS. In addition, we tested two control groups. In the vertex rTMS group (n = 21), rTMS was applied at the same intensity, duration and frequency as in the cerebellar rTMS group, but over the vertex. In the no stimulation group (n = 22) the coil was placed over the cerebellar stimulation site but no pulses were delivered. Results As hypothesised, participants in the cerebellar rTMS group took longer to fixate the target after TMS in the Prediction condition but not in the Control condition (Block-by-Condition interaction: F(1,21) = 8.848, p = 0.007). This interaction was not found in either the vertex rTMS group (F(1,20) = 0.064, p = 0.802) or the no stimulation group (F(1,21) = 2.461, p = 0.132). Conclusions Here, we show that rTMS to the right cerebellum selectively affects linguistic prediction. These results provide additional evidence that the cerebellum plays a role in language and support theoretical accounts that the cerebellum contributes to nonmotor functions, as it does to motor functions, by online prediction. 1. Strick et al (2009). Cerebellum and nonmotor function. Annu Rev Neurosci, 32, 413-134 2. Miall et al (1993). Is the cerebellum a Smith predictor. J Mot Behav, 25, 203-216 3. Marien et al (2001). The lateralised linguistic cerebellum: a review and a new hypothesis. Brain and Language, 79, 580-600 4. Altmann & Kamide (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs. Cognition, 73, 247-264
  • Meyer, A. S. (2012). What's in it for me? What's in it for me? Applying adult speech production models to young learners. Talk presented at a workshop at the University of Leiden. Leiden, The Netherlands. 2012-12.
  • Moers, C., Meyer, A. S., & Janse, E. (2012). Effects of transitional probabilities on word durations in read speech of younger & older speakers. Talk presented at the Workshop Fluent Speech: Combining Cognitive and Educational Approaches, Utrecht Institute of Linguistics. Utrecht, The Netherlands. 2012-11-12 - 2012-11-13.
  • Reifegerste, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). The influence of age on the mental representation of polymorphemic words in Dutch. Talk presented at the Conference on Morphological Complexity. London, UK. 2012-01-13 - 2012-01-15.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Praamstra, P., & Huettig, F. (2012). Object shape representations in the contents of predictions for upcoming words. Talk presented at Psycholinguistics in Flanders [PiF 2012]. Berg en Dal, The Netherlands. 2012-06-06 - 2012-06-07.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., Praamstra, P., & Huettig, F. (2012). The content of predictions: Involvement of object shape representations in the anticipation of upcoming words. Talk presented at the Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen [TeaP 2012]. Mannheim, Germany. 2012-04-04 - 2012-04-06.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2012). Predicting upcoming meaning involves specific contents and domain-general mechanisms. Talk presented at the 18th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2012]. Riva del Garda, Italy. 2012-09-06 - 2012-09-08.

    Abstract

    In sentence comprehension, readers and listeners often anticipate upcoming information (e.g., Altmann & Kamide, 1999). We investigated two aspects of this process, namely 1) what is pre-activated when anticipating an upcoming word (the contents of predictions), and 2) which cognitive mechanisms are involved. The contents of predictions at the level of meaning could be restricted to functional semantic attributes (e.g., edibility; Altmann & Kamide, 1999). However, when words are processed other types of information can also be activated, such as object shape representations. It is unknown whether this type of information is already activated when upcoming words are predicted. Forty-five adult participants listened to predictable words in sentence contexts (e.g., "In 1969 Neil Armstrong was the first man to set foot on the moon.") while looking at visual displays of four objects. Their eye movements were recorded. There were three conditions: target present (e.g., a moon and three distractor objects that were unrelated to the predictable word in terms of semantics, shape, and phonology), shape competitor (e.g., a tomato and three unrelated distractors), and distractors only (e.g., rice and three other unrelated objects). Across lists, the same pictures and sentences were used in the different conditions. We found that participants already showed a significant bias for the target object (moon) over unrelated distractors several seconds before the target was mentioned, demonstrating that they were predicting. Importantly, there was also a smaller but significant shape competitor (tomato) preference starting at about a second before critical word onset, consistent with predictions involving the referent’s shape. The mechanisms of predictions could be specific to language tasks, or language could use processing principles that are also used in other domains of cognition. We investigated whether performance in non-linguistic prediction is related to prediction in language processing, taking an individual differences approach. In addition to the language processing task, the participants performed a simple cueing task (after Posner, Nissen, & Ogden, 1978). They pressed one of two buttons (left/right) to indicate the location of an X symbol on the screen. On half of the trials, the X was preceded by a neutral cue (+). On the other half, an arrow cue pointing left (<) or right (>) indicated the upcoming X's location with 80% validity (i.e., the arrow cue was correct 80% of the time). The SOA between cue and target was 500 ms. Prediction was quantified as the mean response latency difference between the neutral and valid condition. This measure correlated positively with individual participants' anticipatory target and shape competitor preference (r = .27; r = .45), and was a significant predictor of anticipatory looks in linear mixed-effects regression models of the data. Participants who showed more facilitation from the arrow cues predicted to a higher degree in the linguistic task. This suggests that prediction in language processing may use mechanisms that are also used in other domains of cognition. References Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition, 73(3), 247-264. Posner, M. I., Nissen, M. J., & Ogden, W. C. (1978). Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. In: H.L. Pick, & I.J. Saltzman (Eds.), Modes of perceiving and processing information. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Sjerps, M. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Variation in cognitive demands across turn-taking. Poster presented at the 7th International Workshop on Language Production (IWOLP 2012), New York, United States.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Relative clause processing: Linking clause frequency and reading experience. Poster presented at the 11th Psycholinguistics in Flanders Conference [PIF 2012], Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., Bock, K., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Conceptual and grammatical factors in the production of subject-verb agreement. Poster presented at the 7th International Workshop on Language Production (IWOLP 2012), New York, United States.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Conceptual and grammatical factors in the production of subject-verb agreement. Talk presented at The 11th edition of the Psycholinguistics in Flanders conference (PiF). Berg en Dal, The Netherlands. 2012-06-06 - 2012-06-07.
  • Veenstra, A., Acheson, D. J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Life after the spoken preamble completion paradigm. Talk presented at the 33th TABU Dag. Groningen, The Netherlands. 2012-06-18 - 2012-06-19.
  • Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Effects of lexical and structural priming on sentence formulation. Talk presented at the 17th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference [AMLaP 2011]. Paris, France. 2011-08-31 - 2011-09-03.
  • Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Planning messages and sentences with familiar perceptual and syntactic structures. Poster presented at the 17th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology [ESCOP 2011], San Sebastian, Spain.

    Abstract

    If message and sentence planning are closely linked processes, planning scope may vary depending on what speakers want to say and how they say it. We compared speakers’ gaze pattern to pictures in displays eliciting sentences like “The lion and the tiger are above the basket” when speakers were a) more familiar or less familiar with the spatial layout of these displays, and b) more familiar or less familiar with the phrasal structures used in these sentences. Familiarity with spatial layout was induced by presenting prime trials with a similar or dissimilar layout of pictures (“The bell and the nail are above/below the crutch”) before the target trial, and familiarity with sentence structure was manipulated via structural priming (prime trials elicited sentences like “The bell and the nail are above the crutch” or “The bell is above the nail and the crutch”). When describing pictures on target trials, speakers looked earlier at the second object (tiger) when they were familiar with both the spatial layout and sentence structure, but speech onsets were reduced (structural priming) only when both spatial layout and sentence structure were repeated. The results show that linguistic planning is facilitated by congruence between message-level and sentence-level structure.
  • Medaglia, M. T., Porcaro, C., Meyer, A. S., & Krott, A. (2011). Removal of muscle artifacts from EEG recordings by ICA during overt speech production. Poster presented at HBM 2011 - The 17th Annual Meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping, Quebec City, Canada.
  • Meyer, A. S., Ganushchak, L. Y., & Lupker, S. (2011). Sandwich priming effects in picture naming. Talk presented at the Experimental Psychology Society London Meeting. University College London, UK. 2011-01-06 - 2011-01-07.

    Abstract

    Studies of lexical access in speech planning often use priming or interference paradigms, where a target picture is combined with a written prime or distracter word. A difficulty in interpreting the results of studies using interference paradigms with clearly visible distracters is that effects arising during lexical access cannot be distinguished from effects arising during self-monitoring. A difficulty with using masked priming paradigms is that the effects tend to be small and fragile. We report a series of picture naming experiments using both the conventional masked priming procedure and the sandwich priming procedure first used in lexical decision experiments by Lupker and masked prime the participants are briefly (i.e., 33 ms) presented the name of the target picture. Although neither categorically nor phonologically related primes significantly affected picture naming in the traditional masked priming experiments, in the sandwich priming experiments: (a) categorically related primes (e.g. “dog-cat”) interfered more than unrelated distracters with picture naming and (b) phonologically related primes (“mat-cat”) facilitated picture naming. The theoretical implications of these findings will be discussed. Lupker, S.J., & Davis, C.J. (2009). Sandwich priming: A method for overcoming the limitations of masked priming by reducing lexical competition effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 35, 618-639.
  • Meyer, A. S., & Konopka, A. E. (2011). Predictors of sequential object naming: visual layout and working memory capacity. Talk presented at The 52nd meeting of the Psychonomic Society. Seattle. 2011-11-03 - 2011-11-06.
  • Reifegerste, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). The influence of age on the mental representation of polymorphemic words in Dutch. Poster presented at the 13th Winter Conference of the Dutch Psychonomic Society, Egmond aan Zee, The Netherlands.
  • Reifegerste, J., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). The influence of working memory on the mental representation of polymorphemic words in Dutch. Talk presented at the Conference on Morphological Complexity. London. 2012-01-13 - 2012-01-15.

    Abstract

    Models of the mental representation of morphologically complex words traditionally fall into one of two categories, Single-Route or Dual-Route models. The former further distinguish between Full-Listing (e.g. Butterworth, 1983) and Decomposition (e.g. Taft & Forster, 1976), while the latter assume different systems governing the access of mono- vs. polymorphemic words (e.g. Pinker & Prince, 1994; Pinker & Ullman, 2002). One of the main arguments against decomposition and continuous online computations is the cognitive resources this process would require. Turning this reasoning around, taxing someone's working memory capacities should then uniquely affect the computation of bimorphemic verb forms. We tested this hypothesis on 48 Dutch native speakers with a lexical decision task, comparing reaction times for Dutch regular past tense forms to frequency-matched irregular past tense forms, both under low and under high cognitive load. We found that frequency influenced reactions to monomorphemic but not to bimorphemic forms (F(1, 47) = 4.734, p = .035), favoring a listing account for the former but a computational procedure for the latter forms. This interaction, however, was present only for a certain group of people (F(1, 23) = 6.279, p = .02), namely those whose reaction times were hardly affected by the load manipulation and who thus may be thought of as having larger working memory capacities. On the other hand, participants who showed a strong load effect had no interaction between number of morphemes and frequency (F(1, 23) = .575, ns), indicating that they process monomorphemic and bimorphemic forms in a similar manner. It seems that cognitive capacities influence the storage of and access to polymorphemic verb forms. While people with greater working memory skills use these resources to compute morphologically complex inflections on-line, people with smaller cognitive capacities seem to rely on a list-like storage for bimorphemic forms as well.
  • Rommers, J., Huettig, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Task-dependency in the activation of visual representations during language processing. Poster presented at Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen [TaeP 2011], Halle (Saale), Germany.
  • Rommers, J., Meyer, A. S., & Huettig, F. (2011). The timing of the on-line activation of visual shape information during sentence processing. Poster presented at the 17th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2011], Paris, France.
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Individual differences in picture naming speed: Contribution of executive control. Poster presented at The 17th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology [ESCOP 2011], San Sebastian, Spain.

    Abstract

    Speakers clearly differ in how quickly they can retrieve words from the mental lexicon, but little is known about the sources of this variability. The present study investigated the relationship between speakers’ executive control abilities and their speed of picture naming. In two experiments, adult speakers of British English named line drawings of objects and actions. Three main components of executive control - updating, shifting of attention, and inhibiting - were assessed using the operation-span, number-letter shifting, and stop-signal task, respectively (see Myake et al.,2000 ). Reaction times (RT) to action and object pictures were highly correlated. Ex-Gaussian analyses of the RT distributions showed that the speakers’ updating scores correlated with the tau parameter of the RT distributions, i.e. predicted the proportions of slow responses in action and object naming. The inhibiting scores correlated with the mean RTs, whereas the scores obtained in the number-letter shifting task were uncorrelated to the RTs. These results indicate that the executive control abilities of updating and inhibiting contribute to the speed of naming objects and actions. Theories of word production may require modification to take account of these findings.
  • Stregapede, F., Meyer, A. S., & Miall, C. R. (2011). Reading between the lines: Inference processes in the online comprehension of symbolic haiku. Poster presented at ESCOP 2011 - 17th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology, San Sebastian/Donostia (SP).

    Abstract

    a bitter rain – two silences beneath the one umbrella Is the connotative meaning of texts readily available or is it gleaned at an extra cognitive cost? The eye-movements of 31 English native speakers (10 male, mean age 21 years) were recorded while reading 24 haiku, 12 in the original/symbolic version, and 12 in a modified version where the most symbolic word (the keyword ‘bitter’ in the example) was replaced by a more literal word (‘loud’) reducing the text’s symbolic purport. The effects of keyword substitutions were measured globally, comparing total reading times for the two haiku types, and locally, examining the first pass gaze durations and dwell times on a word closely connected to the keyword, the referent ‘silences’, and on the last word, ‘umbrella’, to examine wrap-up processes. First pass duration showed no effects of the substitution. However, dwell time on referents and last-word regions, and total reading time were significantly longer for the original than for the altered haiku, suggesting that the connotative meaning of the texts was not available immediately but only through re-reading of the texts. The implications of these findings are discussed in relation to the literature on the processing of inferences in symbolic texts.
  • Stregapede, F., Meyer, A. S., & Miall, C. R. (2011). Taking a second or a third look at symbolic but not at literal haiku: An eye-tracking study. Poster presented at ECEM 2011 - 16th European Conference on Eye Movements, Marseille (FR).

    Abstract

    a bitter rain – two silences beneath the one umbrella Is the connotative meaning of a text readily available or is it accessed only after re-examining a text? Thirtyone English native speakers (10 male, mean age 21) read 24 haiku, 12 in their original/symbolic version and 12 in a version in which the most symbolic word (the keyword "bitter" in the example above) was replaced by a more literal word ("loud"), reducing the text’s symbolic purport. Participants' eye movements were recorded using the eye-tracker EyeLink 1000. The effect of the word substitution was measured globally, by comparing the total reading times for the two haiku types, and locally, by examining first pass duration and dwell time on a word closely connected to the keyword (the referent, "silences") and on the last word ("umbrella"), as this might show wrap-up processes. First pass durations showed no effects of the substitution. However, total reading time and dwell time on both the referent and the last-word regions were significantly longer for haiku with the original keyword than for haiku with the altered keyword. These findings suggest that the texts’ connotative meaning was not available immediately but only through re-reading of the texts.
  • Van de Velde, M., Konopka, A. E., & Meyer, A. S. (2011). Experience with a sentence structure modulates planning strategies—an eye-tracking experiment. Poster presented at the 13th Winter Conference of the Dutch Psychonomic Society [NVP], Egmond aan Zee, the Netherlands.
  • Vuong, L., Meyer, A. S., & Cristansen, M. (2011). Simultaneous online tracking of adjacent and nonadjacent dependencies in statistical learning. Poster presented at The 17th Meeting of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology [ESCOP 2011], San Sebastian, Spain.

    Abstract

    When children learn their native language, they have to deal with a confusing array of dependencies between various elements in an utterance. Some of these dependencies may be adjacent to one another whereas others can be separated by considerable intervening material. In this study, we investigate whether both types of dependencies can be learned together, similarly to the task facing young children. Statistical learning of adjacent dependencies (probability = .17) and non-adjacent dependencies (probability = 1.0) was assessed in two experiments using a modified serial-reaction-time task. The results showed (i) increasing online sensitivity to both dependency types during training, (ii) better nonadjacency than adjacency learning, and (iii) nonadjacency learning being highly correlated with adjacency learning, suggesting that adjacency and non-adjacency learning can occur in parallel and might be subserved by a common statistical learning mechanism. An overnight break between two training sessions helped the online learning performance of slower learners to approach that of faster learners, but the same amount of training without such a break (a 15-min interval) did not, suggesting that memory consolidation may play a role in statistical learning of complex statistical patterns, especially for slower learners.
  • Chu, M., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Name-picture verification as a control measure for object naming: Data from British English speakers. Poster presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010, Manchester, UK.

    Abstract

    The name-picture verification task is often used to assess the difficulty of prelexical processes (object recognition and semantic access) during picture naming. However, whether to use responses from word-picture match or from mismatch trials to index the difficulty of pre-lexical processes is debated. Levelt (2002) argued for the use of mismatch trials because on match trials the printed object name might facilitate picture recognition. However, in a study with speakers of Spanish Stadthagen-Gonzalez et al. (2009) showed that visual and conceptual properties of objects only correlated with the latencies of match responses but not with those of mismatch responses and therefore advocated the use of match responses. The present study aimed to replicate Stadthagen- Gonzalez et al. (2009) findings using native British English speakers and English norms for non-lexical and lexical variables. We replicated the finding that non-lexical variables affected the speed of match, but not mismatch responses. However, in addition, we found that lexical variables also affected the speed of match responses, which means that these latencies need to be interpreted with caution. In other words, neither match nor mismatch responses seem ideally suited to assess the difficulty of pre-lexical processes in picture naming. Levelt, W. J. M. (2002). Picture naming and word frequency. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 663–671. Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., Damian, M. F., Pérez, M. A., Bowers, J. S., & Marín, J. (2009). Name-picture verification as a control measure for object naming: A task analysis and norms for a
  • Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Lexical representations of SMS shortcuts. Talk presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010. Manchester, UK. 2010-07-07 - 2010-07-09.

    Abstract

    As the popularity of sending messages electronically increased, so did the necessity of conveying messages more efficiently. A popular way of increasing efficiency is to use shortcuts such as gr8 “great” or cu “see you”. The aim of the study was to investigate whether such abbreviations have their own lexical representations or whether they are recorded into the words they stand for. We used associative masked and overt priming in lexical decision tasks. Primes were text shortcuts and their corresponding words. Targets were words that were associatively related to the prime (e.g., cu/see you – GOODBYE), associatively related to a part of the prime (e.g., cu/see you – LOOK) or unrelated to the prime and any part of the prime (e.g., 4u/for you – GOODBYE; 4u/for you – LOOK). In both tasks, responses were faster to targets preceded by related compared to unrelated primes (shortcuts and words). In the overt priming task, we in addition found faster responses when the target was related to a part of the prime. However, this effect was present only for word but not for shortcut primes. These results indicate that shortcuts have their own lexical representations that are independent of the representations of the individual words they replace.
  • Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Ondrzk vd nwe taal. Talk presented at NWO Conference Bessensap 2010. The Hague, The Netherlands. 2010-06-07.

    Abstract

    Ik laat zien dat het gebruik van sms- en msn-afkortingen geen schadelijke gevolgen heeft op het begrip van geschreven taal zoals blijkt uit een reeks van experimenten. Sterker nog, afkortingen hebben een eigen representatie in ons mentale woordenboek (bijvoorbeeld ‘iig’) en worden niet vertaald naar woorden waaruit ze bestaan (bijvoorbeeld ‘in ieder geval’). Het gebruik van korte tekstberichten via mobiele telefoons en e-mail neemt steeds meer toe en daardoor worden steeds meer afkortingen en acroniemen in de taal gebruikt. Momenteel worden er wereldwijd naar schatting 2000 miljard sms-jes per jaar verstuurd. In Nederland en België sms’t ongeveer 80% van de jongeren regelmatig, en ongeveer 70% van hen gebruikt sms-taal (Daniels, 2009). Het gebruik van afkortingen heeft de schuld gekregen van de verloedering van de Nederlandse taal, iets dat men terug zou zien in de kwaliteit van het Nederlands in e-mails en brieven. Verder gebruik van afkortingen zou de communicatie verslechteren tussen de gebruikers van afkortingen en niet gebruikers, bijvoorbeeld tussen studenten en docenten. De vraag is of dat echt zo is. Uit mijn onderzoek blijkt dat het begrijpen van een tekst niet leidt onder het gebruik van afkortingen in de zinnen
  • Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Processing of SMS shortcuts [Keynote lecture]. Talk presented at 20th Anéla Juniorendag. Leiden, The Netherlands. 2010-01-29.
  • Meyer, A. S., Chu, M., & Ganushchak, L. (2010). Overt and masked semantic and phonological priming of picture naming. Talk presented at Competition Effects in Language Production, Workshop. Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
  • Mortensen, L., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Naming associated objects: Evidence for parallel processing. Talk presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010. Manchester, UK. 2010-07-07 - 2010-07-09.

    Abstract

    Earlier research has shown that speakers naming object pairs can retrieve their names in parallel, but often fail to do so. The conditions for the occurrence of parallel object processing are largely unknown. We examined how associations between objects affected the speakers’ processing strategies. In Experiment 1, participants named object triplets in a left-right-bottom order. During the saccade towards the right object (interloper), it was replaced by a new object (target). We varied the relationship between interloper and target (conceptually related or unrelated) and between interloper and left object (categorically related, associated, unrelated). Target gaze durations were shorter after related than unrelated interlopers. However, this preview effect was independent of the interloper-left object relationship, suggesting that the left and right object were processed sequentially. In Experiments 2 and 3, participants named pairs of associated or unrelated objects. The left objects were repeated several times in each test block to facilitate their processing. Gaze durations for the left objects were longer in associated than unrelated pairs, consistent with parallel processing of the associated objects and interference among them. We will discuss how the difficulty of processing the objects and the relationship between them jointly determine speakers’ processing strategy.
  • Rommers, J., Huettig, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Task-dependency in the activation of visual representations during language comprehension. Poster presented at The Embodied Mind: Perspectives and Limitations, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
  • Rommers, J., Huettig, F., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Task-dependent activation of visual representations during language comprehension. Poster presented at The 16th Annual Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing [AMLaP 2010], York, UK.
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2010). Naming speed is negatively related to working memory capacity. Poster presented at the Meeting at the University of Manchester of the Experimental Psychology Society 2010, Manchester, UK.

    Abstract

    Individuals differ greatly on how efficiently they can retrieve words from the mental lexicon in spoken word production. Little is known about how this individual difference might be related to individual difference in other cognitive abilities. The present study investigated the relationship between individuals’ working memory capacity and their efficiency of lexical access. Speeded naming of drawings of actions and objects was used to measure the efficiency of lexical access. To assess working memory capacity, we used the Operation Span (OSPAN) task (Turner & Engle, 1989), in which participants memorize unrelated words while making judgments on simple mathematic operations. We found that action naming response time (RT) was significantly longer than object naming RT. More importantly, there was a significant negative correlation between the action naming RT and the OSPAN score. There was no correlation between object naming RT and the OSPAN score. Ex-Gaussian analyses of the RT distributions revealed that the negative relationship between OSPAN and RT was evident throughout the distributions (i.e., reflected in the Gaussian part of the distribution). These results suggest that when words are difficult to select (our verbcondition) people with larger working memory capacity outperform people with lower working memory capacity. Turner, M. L., & Engle. R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127-154.
  • Ganushchak, L. Y., Krott, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2009). GR8 msg! Are these actual words?. Talk presented at the 16th Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology {ESCOP 2009]. Krakow, Poland.
  • Cooke, N., Russell, M., & Meyer, A. S. (2004). Evaluation of hidden Markov models robustness in uncovering focus of visual attention from noisy eye-tracker data. Poster presented at Eye Tracking Research and Applications Symposium 2004 (ETRA 2004), San Antonio, Texas.

    Abstract

    A robust way to unconver the focus of visual attention from (simulated) noisy eye tracking data provided by the hidden Markov model was discussed. It was found that a hidden simi-Markov model (HSMM) with explicit state duration PDF representing task-constrained visual attention was more stable and accurate to represent visual attention duration. HSMM used an additional Gaussian component to the observation distribution PDF with larger standard deviation to ensure less differentiation between eye movement positions for away from the object. Analysis shows that HMM and HSMM performed better in terms of accuracy and instability than the baseline non-HMM method.

Share this page