
Group leader
Post doc
PhD students
MSc students
Pedro Alonso Gonzalez
Julia Niehaus
Research assistant
Student assistant
Rafael Zampakas
Alumni
Chin Yang Shapland
Marjolein van Donkelaar
Mariska Barendse
Laurence Howe (co-supervision with University of Bristol, Professor George Davey Smith and Dr Sarah Lewis)
Janne Vermeulen
Jeffrey van der Ven
Mitchell Olislagers
Celeste Figaroa
Tanguy Rubat du Mérac
Simone van den Bedem
Fenja Schlag
Anh Nguyen
Paola Moreno Ancalmo
Displaying 121 - 140 of 15536
-
Coopmans, C. W., De Hoop, H., Tezcan, F., Hagoort, P., & Martin, A. E. (2025). Language-specific neural dynamics extend syntax into the time domain. PLOS Biology, 23: e3002968. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3002968.
Abstract
Studies of perception have long shown that the brain adds information to its sensory analysis of the physical environment. A touchstone example for humans is language use: to comprehend a physical signal like speech, the brain must add linguistic knowledge, including syntax. Yet, syntactic rules and representations are widely assumed to be atemporal (i.e., abstract and not bound by time), so they must be translated into time-varying signals for speech comprehension and production. Here, we test 3 different models of the temporal spell-out of syntactic structure against brain activity of people listening to Dutch stories: an integratory bottom-up parser, a predictive top-down parser, and a mildly predictive left-corner parser. These models build exactly the same structure but differ in when syntactic information is added by the brain—this difference is captured in the (temporal distribution of the) complexity metric “incremental node count.” Using temporal response function models with both acoustic and information-theoretic control predictors, node counts were regressed against source-reconstructed delta-band activity acquired with magnetoencephalography. Neural dynamics in left frontal and temporal regions most strongly reflect node counts derived by the top-down method, which postulates syntax early in time, suggesting that predictive structure building is an important component of Dutch sentence comprehension. The absence of strong effects of the left-corner model further suggests that its mildly predictive strategy does not represent Dutch language comprehension well, in contrast to what has been found for English. Understanding when the brain projects its knowledge of syntax onto speech, and whether this is done in language-specific ways, will inform and constrain the development of mechanistic models of syntactic structure building in the brain. -
Wu, S.-S., Pan, H., Sheldrick, R. C., Shao, J., Liu, X.-M., Zheng, S.-S., Pereira Soares, S. M., Zhang, L., Sun, J., Xu, P., Chen, S.-H., Sun, T., Pang, J.-W., Wu, N., Feng, Y.-C., Chen, N.-R., Zhang, Y.-T., & Jiang, F. (2025). Development and validation of the Parent-Reported Indicator of Developmental Evaluation for Chinese Children (PRIDE) tool. World Journal of Pediatrics, 21, 183-191. doi:10.1007/s12519-025-00878-7.
Abstract
Background
Developmental delay (DD) poses challenges to children's overall development, necessitating early detection and intervention. Existing screening tools in China focus mainly on children with developmental issues in two or more domains, diagnosed as global developmental delay (GDD). However, the recent rise of early childhood development (ECD) concepts has expanded the focus to include not only those with severe brain development impairments but also children who lag in specific domains due to various social-environmental factors, with the aim of promoting positive development through active intervention. To support this approach, corresponding screening tools need to be developed.
Methods
The current study used a two-phase design to develop and validate the Parent-Reported Indicator of Developmental Evaluation for Chinese Children (PRIDE) tool. In Phase 1, age-specific milestone forms for PRIDE were created through a survey conducted in urban and rural primary care clinics across four economic regions in China. In Phase 2, PRIDE was validated in a community-based sample. Sensitivity and specificity of both PRIDE and Ages and Stages Questionnaires (ASQ)-3 were estimated using inverse probability weights (IPW) and multiple imputation (MI) to address planned and unplanned missing data.
Results
In Phase 1 involving a total of 1160 participants aged 1 to 48 months, 63 items were selected from the initial item pool to create 10 age-specific PRIDE forms. Our Phase 2 study included 777 children within the same age range. PRIDE demonstrated an estimated sensitivity and specificity of 83.3% [95% confidence interval (CI): 56.8%–100.0%] and 84.9% (95% CI: 82.8%–86.9%) in the identification of DD.
Conclusion
The findings suggest that PRIDE holds promise as a sensitive tool for detecting DD in community settings.Additional information
supplementary information -
Papoutsi, C., Tourtouri, E. N., Piai, V., Lampe, L. F., & Meyer, A. S. (2025). Fast and slow errors: What naming latencies of errors reveal about the interplay of attentional control and word planning in speeded picture naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/xlm0001472.
Abstract
Speakers sometimes produce lexical errors, such as saying “salt” instead of “pepper.” This study aimed to better understand the origin of lexical errors by assessing whether they arise from a hasty selection and premature decision to speak (premature selection hypothesis) or from momentary attentional disengagement from the task (attentional lapse hypothesis). We analyzed data from a speeded picture naming task (Lampe et al., 2023) and investigated whether lexical errors are produced as fast as target (i.e., correct) responses, thus arising from premature selection, or whether they are produced more slowly than target responses, thus arising from lapses of attention. Using ex-Gaussian analyses, we found that lexical errors were slower than targets in the tail, but not in the normal part of the response time distribution, with the tail effect primarily resulting from errors that were not coordinates, that is, members of the target’s semantic category. Moreover, we compared the coordinate errors and target responses in terms of their word-intrinsic properties and found that they were overall more frequent, shorter, and acquired earlier than targets. Given the present findings, we conclude that coordinate errors occur due to a premature selection but in the context of intact attentional control, following the same lexical constraints as targets, while other errors, given the variability in their nature, may vary in their origin, with one potential source being lapses of attention. -
Karaca, F. (2025). On knowing what lies ahead: The interplay of prediction, experience, and proficiency. PhD Thesis, Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen.
Additional information
link to Radboud Repository -
Mamus, E., Speed, L. J., Ortega, G., Majid, A., & Özyürek, A. (2024). Gestures reveal how visual experience shapes concepts in blind and sighted individuals. Talk presented at the 14th International Conference on Iconicity in Language and Literature (ILL14). Catania. 2024-05-30 - 2024-06-01.
-
Cheung, C.-Y., Kirby, S., & Raviv, L. (2024). The role of gender, personality traits, and social biases in shaping linguistic accommodation: An experimental approach. Poster presented at the IMPRS Conference 2024, Nijmegen, the Netherlands.
-
Hollowell, A. C., Gui, A., Hannigan, L., Corfield, E. C., Morgan, M. J., Middeldorp, C., St Pourcain, B., Arichi, T., Dudbrige, F., Johnson, M. H., Havdahl, A. K. S., & Ronald, A. (2024). Genome-wide association meta-analysis of infant fussiness in the first year. Poster presented at the 54th Annual Meeting of the Behavior Genetics Association, London.
-
Schreiner, M. S., Zettersten, M., Bergmann, C., Frank, M. C., Fritzsche, T., Gonzalez-Gomez, N., Hamlin, K., Kartushina, N., Kellier, D. J., Mani, N., Mayor, J., Saffran, J., Shukla, M., Silverstein, P., Soderstrom, M., & Lippold, M. (2024). Limited evidence of test-retest reliability in infant-directed speech preference in a large pre-registered infant experiment. Developmental Science, 27(6): e13551. doi:10.1111/desc.13551.
Abstract
est-retest reliability—establishing that measurements remain consistent across multiple testing sessions—is critical to measuring, understanding, and predicting individual differences in infant language development. However, previous attempts to establish measurement reliability in infant speech perception tasks are limited, and reliability of frequently used infant measures is largely unknown. The current study investigated the test-retest reliability of infants’ preference for infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech in a large sample (N = 158) in the context of the ManyBabies1 collaborative research project. Labs were asked to bring in participating infants for a second appointment retesting infants on their preference for infant-directed speech. This approach allowed us to estimate test-retest reliability across three different methods used to investigate preferential listening in infancy: the head-turn preference procedure, central fixation, and eye-tracking. Overall, we found no consistent evidence of test-retest reliability in measures of infants’ speech preference (overall r = 0.09, 95% CI [−0.06,0.25]). While increasing the number of trials that infants needed to contribute for inclusion in the analysis revealed a numeric growth in test-retest reliability, it also considerably reduced the study’s effective sample size. Therefore, future research on infant development should take into account that not all experimental measures may be appropriate for assessing individual differences between infants. -
Wilms, V. (2024). The effect of competing speech on word comprehension and production. Poster presented at the Highlights in the Language Sciences Conference 2024, Nijmegen, The Netherlands.
-
Josserand, M., Pellegrino, F., Grosseck, O., Dediu, D., & Raviv, L. (2024). Adapting to individual differences: An experimental study of variation in language evolution. In J. Nölle, L. Raviv, K. E. Graham, S. Hartmann, Y. Jadoul, M. Josserand, T. Matzinger, K. Mudd, M. Pleyer, A. Slonimska, & S. Wacewicz (
Eds. ), The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 15th International Conference (EVOLANG XV) (pp. 286-289). Nijmegen: The Evolution of Language Conferences. -
Long, M., & Rubio-Fernández, P. (2024). Beyond typicality: Lexical category affects the use and processing of color words. Poster presented at the 46th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci 2024), Rotterdam, The Netherlands.
-
Bujok, R., Meyer, A. S., & Bosker, H. R. (2024). Beat gestures can affect audiovisual lexical stress perception immediately. Talk presented at he 29th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP 2024). Edinburgh, Scotland. 2024-09-04 - 2024-09-07.
-
Maran, M., Rossen, R., Uilenreef, R., & Bosker, H. R. (2024). The timing of an avatar’s gestures differentially influences lexical stress perception in normal and simulated cochlear implant hearing conditions. Poster presented at the 29th Architectures and Mechanisms for Language Processing Conference (AMLaP 2024), Edinburgh, Scotland.
-
Wang, J., Schiller, N. O., & Verdonschot, R. G. (2024). Word and morpheme frequency effects in naming Mandarin Chinese compounds: More than a replication. Brain and Language, 259: 105496. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2024.105496.
Abstract
The question whether compound words are stored in our mental lexicon in a decomposed or full-listing way prompted Janssen and colleagues (2008) to investigate the representation of compounds using word and morpheme frequencies manipulations. Our study replicated their study using a new set of stimuli from a spoken corpus and incorporating EEG data for a more detailed investigation. In the current study, despite ERP analyses revealing no word frequency or morpheme frequency effects across conditions, behavioral outcomes indicated that Mandarin compounds are not sensitive to word frequency. Instead, the response times highlighted a morpheme frequency effect in naming Mandarin compounds, which contrasted with the findings of Janssen and colleagues. These findings challenge the full-listing model and instead support the decompositional model. -
Ten Oever, S., & Martin, A. E. (2024). Interdependence of “what” and “when” in the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 36(1), 167-186. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_02067.
Abstract
From a brain's-eye-view, when a stimulus occurs and what it is are interrelated aspects of interpreting the perceptual world. Yet in practice, the putative perceptual inferences about sensory content and timing are often dichotomized and not investigated as an integrated process. We here argue that neural temporal dynamics can influence what is perceived, and in turn, stimulus content can influence the time at which perception is achieved. This computational principle results from the highly interdependent relationship of what and when in the environment. Both brain processes and perceptual events display strong temporal variability that is not always modeled; we argue that understanding—and, minimally, modeling—this temporal variability is key for theories of how the brain generates unified and consistent neural representations and that we ignore temporal variability in our analysis practice at the peril of both data interpretation and theory-building. Here, we review what and when interactions in the brain, demonstrate via simulations how temporal variability can result in misguided interpretations and conclusions, and outline how to integrate and synthesize what and when in theories and models of brain computation. -
Kakimoto, N., Wongratwanich, P., Shimamoto, H., Kitisubkanchana, J., Tsujimoto, T., Shimabukuro, K., Verdonschot, R. G., Hasegawa, Y., & Murakami, S. (2024). Comparison of T2 values of the displaced unilateral disc and retrodiscal tissue of temporomandibular joints and their implications. Scientific Reports, 14: 1705. doi:10.1038/s41598-024-52092-6.
Abstract
Unilateral anterior disc displacement (uADD) has been shown to affect the contralateral joints qualitatively. This study aims to assess the quantitative T2 values of the articular disc and retrodiscal tissue of patients with uADD at 1.5 Tesla (T). The study included 65 uADD patients and 17 volunteers. The regions of interest on T2 maps were evaluated. The affected joints demonstrated significantly higher articular disc T2 values (31.5 ± 3.8 ms) than those of the unaffected joints (28.9 ± 4.5 ms) (P < 0.001). For retrodiscal tissue, T2 values of the unaffected (37.8 ± 5.8 ms) and affected joints (41.6 ± 7.1 ms) were significantly longer than those of normal volunteers (34.4 ± 3.2 ms) (P < 0.001). Furthermore, uADD without reduction (WOR) joints (43.3 ± 6.8 ms) showed statistically higher T2 values than the unaffected joints of both uADD with reduction (WR) (33.9 ± 3.8 ms) and uADDWOR (38.9 ± 5.8 ms), and the affected joints of uADDWR (35.8 ± 4.4 ms). The mean T2 value of the unaffected joints of uADDWOR was significantly longer than that of healthy volunteers (P < 0.001). These results provided quantitative evidence for the influence of the affected joints on the contralateral joints. -
Wang, N., Grüning, D. J., van der Burght, C. L., Fried, E. I., Steele, J., Columbus, S., & Chuan-Peng, H. (2024). Four ontological challenges to constructs in psychology. PsyArXiv Preprints. doi:10.31234/osf.io/hyfbn.
Abstract
All areas of psychology deal with constructs in one way or another. The use of psychological constructs presents four distinct challenges that are central to address: describing, defining, deploying, and distinguishing constructs. Refining a psychological construct refers to an iterative process of dealing with these challenges -
Bujok, R., Peeters, D., Meyer, A. S., & Bosker, H. R. (2024). Beating stress: Evidence for recalibration of word stress perception. PsyArXiv Preprints. doi:10.31234/osf.io/9sua6.
Abstract
Speech is inherently variable, requiring listeners to apply adaptation mechanisms to deal with the variability. A proposed perceptual adaptation mechanism is recalibration, whereby listeners learn to adjust cognitive representations of speech sounds based on disambiguating contextual information. Most studies on the role of recalibration in speech perception have focused on variability in particular speech segments (e.g., consonants/vowels), and speech has mostly been studied in isolation. However, speech is often accompanied by visual bodily signals like hand gestures and is thus multimodal. Moreover, variability in speech extends beyond segmental aspects alone and also affects prosodic aspects, like lexical stress. We currently do not understand well how listeners adjust their representations of lexical stress patterns to different speakers. In three experiments, we investigated recalibration of lexical stress perception, driven by lexico-orthographical information (Experiment 1) and by manual beat gestures (Experiments 2-3). Across experiments, we observed that these types of disambiguating information (presented during an initial brief exposure phase) lead listeners to adjust their representations of lexical stress, with lasting consequences for subsequent spoken word recognition (in an audio-only test phase). However, evidence for generalization of this recalibration to segmentally different words was mixed as it was found only in the final experiment. These results highlight that recalibration is a plausible mechanism for suprasegmental speech adaption in everyday communication and show that even the timing of simple hand gestures can have a lasting effect on auditory speech perception. -
Stivers, T., Chalfoun, A., & Rossi, G. (2024). To err is human but to persist is diabolical: Toward a theory of interactional policing. Frontiers in Sociology: Sociological Theory, 9: 1369776. doi:10.3389/fsoc.2024.1369776.
Abstract
Social interaction is organized around norms and preferences that guide our construction of actions and our interpretation of those of others, creating a reflexive moral order. Sociological theory suggests two possibilities for the type of moral order that underlies the policing of interactional norm and preference violations: a morality that focuses on the nature of violations themselves and a morality that focuses on the positioning of actors as they maintain their conduct comprehensible, even when they depart from norms and preferences. We find that actors are more likely to reproach interactional violations for which an account is not provided by the transgressor, and that actors weakly reproach or let pass first offenses while more strongly policing violators who persist in bad behavior. Based on these findings, we outline a theory of interactional policing that rests not on the nature of the violation but rather on actors' moral positioning. -
Cheung, C.-Y., Kirby, S., & Raviv, L. (2024). The role of gender, personality traits, and social biases in shaping linguistic accommodation: An experimental approach. Talk presented at the International Conference on the Evolution of Language (Evolang 2024). Madison, WI, USA. 2024-05-18 - 2024-05-21.
Share this page